ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Method Article
Revised

A novel variable delay Go/No-Go task to study attention, motivation and working memory in the head-fixed rodent

[version 2; peer review: 2 approved]
PUBLISHED 19 Mar 2014
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

Abstract

In order to parse the causal elements underlying complex behaviors and decision-making processes, appropriate behavioral methods must be developed and used in concurrence with molecular, pharmacological, and electrophysiological approaches. Presented is a protocol for a novel Go/No-Go behavioral paradigm to study the brain attention and motivation/reward circuitry in awake, head-restrained rodents. This experimental setup allows: (1) Pharmacological and viral manipulation of various brain regions via targeted guide cannula; (2) Optogenetic cell-type specific activation and silencing with simultaneous electrophysiological recording and; (3) Repeated electrophysiological single and multiple unit recordings during ongoing behavior. The task consists of three components. The subject first makes an observing response by initiating a trial by lever pressing in response to distinctive Go or No-Go tones.  Then, after a variable delay period, the subject is presented with a challenge period cued by white noise during which they must respond with a lever press for the Go condition or withhold from lever pressing for the duration of the cue in the No-Go condition. After correctly responding during the challenge period (Challenge) and a brief delay, a final reward tone of the same frequency as the initiation tone is presented and sucrose reward delivery is available and contingent upon lever pressing. Here, we provide a novel procedure and validating data set that allows researchers to study and manipulate components of behavior such as attention, motivation, impulsivity, and reward-related working memory during an ongoing operant behavioral task while limiting interference from non task-related behaviors.

Keywords

Go/No-Go, attention, motivation, working memory, head-fixed rodent

Revised Amendments from Version 1

We thank the reviewers for their feedback. We have addressed the issue of novelty in the introduction section of the article. Our task allows for simultaneous measurement of the subject's motivational level, impulsivity and working memory. The present behavioral task utilizes skilled forelimb movements in rodents.
We have included a new figure (Figure 5) and figure legend. This figure shows between session responding before and after achieving criteria (A) and within session responding after achieving criteria (B). The purpose of this figure is to show stable challenge period responding after criteria is met, as well as stable challenge period responding within an individual training session.
We have included a short description in the introduction section of the article demonstrating that the Go/No-go task has been applied to electrophysiological recordings, lesion studies and pharmacological studies with references to earlier studies. The main purpose of this article is to describe the utility of the task for studying different behavioral components and demonstrate the time course to achievement of criteria. Therefore, we did not include any specific Go/No-go historical anecdotes in the article but we appreciated the suggestion.

To read any peer review reports and author responses for this article, follow the "read" links in the Open Peer Review table.

Introduction

The use of operant behavioral tasks that utilize the head-immobilized condition in rodents have some advantages over tasks that occur under freely moving conditions by limiting the range of possible non task-relevant behaviors and distractors. While the head-restrained behaving rodent has been examined for many years, the relevant literature is minimal in comparison to similar behavioral tasks involving freely moving animals. Head immobilization is a traditional and common method in electrophysiological studies utilizing behaving monkeys, in which skilled intentional forelimb movements (e.g. grasping) are widely used as conditional responses. Compared with monkey studies, despite a growing number of studies that utilize head-fixed rodents, very few experiments have assessed psychological and physiological processes simultaneously occurring during operant behavioral tasks that require intentional skilled movements (e.g. lever pressing) as conditional responses in the awake, head-restrained rodent1,2.

The Go/No-Go task, a canonical paradigm in animal behavior and psychology, requires the subject to initiate an operant conditioned response during one stimulus (CS+) and withhold from such a response during the presence of the opposite type of stimulus (CS-)1,3. Variations of the Go/No-go task have long been applied to electrophysiological, lesion and pharmacological studies47. This type of behavioral task has a high level of construct validity and clinical utility when studying the brain motivation/reward pathways and behaviors such as impulsivity. Impulsivity and motivational deficits are complex behavioral phenotypes implicated in a number of psychiatric disorders including attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), mania and depression associated with bipolar disorder, schizophrenia, pathological gambling, borderline personality disorder, and substance abuse3.

The Go/No-Go task we developed allows for simultaneous measurement of the subject’s motivational level, impulsivity and short-term memory. The task consists of four periods: initiation, delay, challenge and reward. First, rats are required to press the lever in response to distinct Go or No-Go tones to initiate a trial (initiation). Performance during this period can be used as a measurement of motivational level. After initiating a trial, a variable delay follows and the rats must maintain information about the cue (tone frequency). After the delay, rats must make a correct Go or No-Go response according to the preceding tones (challenge). A delay period can be used to assess short-term memory and performance during the challenge period can be used as a measure of impulsivity. Finally, a reward tone of the same frequency as the initiation tone is presented following a correct response during challenge period. Data obtained from these studies provide the opportunity for development of novel future pharmacotherapies and behavioral interventions8.

The head-immobilized rodent Go/No-Go task we have developed is a procedure that provides the framework for performing repeated electrophysiological recordings and simultaneous molecular and pharmacologic interventions during ongoing operant behavioral tasks. In addition, this technique allows for accurate stereotaxic localization during electrophysiological recordings, daily anesthesia-free recordings in various brain regions, rapid electrode repositioning during chronic recording sessions, and high quality stabilized recordings8. With the advent of optogenetics, the head-restrained procedure allows for these recordings to be coupled with cell-type specific silencing and activation, free from electrophysiological artifacts9.

Methods

Animals

Four male Sprague Dawley rats and 28 male Long Evans rats (8 weeks and 200–300 g at time of surgery), bred at the University of Colorado Boulder, were used for all experiments. Animals were maintained on a 12 h reverse light/dark cycle (lights off at 07:00). The behavioral experiments were conducted during the dark period. Animals were singly housed and water-restricted, while food was available ad libitum for the duration of the experiment. All procedures were in compliance with animal care standards set forth by the Institute for Animal Care and Use Committee at the University of Colorado Boulder.

Surgical preparation

Animals were stereotaxically affixed with stainless steel head-caps prior to any behavioral training. Rats were deeply anesthetized with an intraperitoneal injection of ketamine-xylazine (Sigma-Aldrich; 80 mg/ml ketamine/6 mg/ml xylazine) and restrained on a custom stereotaxic device (Old School Industries, CO, USA). Head fur was shaved and a rostro-caudal incision was made from the region roughly 10 mm anterior to bregma, extending 5–7 mm posterior to lamba. Connective tissue on the skull was carefully removed and temporal muscles were retracted from the dorsolateral region of the skull. Vasculature was cauterized using 30% hydrogen peroxide (Sigma-Aldrich, USA) and quickly rinsed with sterilized saline. Eight self-tapping screws (Small Parts, IN, USA) were implanted on the dorsal surface of the skull and four screws were implanted on the lateral region of the skull to confer head-cap stability and strength. After screws were implanted, the skull was sterilized using povidone-iodine (9%) (Sigma-Aldrich, USA) then desiccated with sterile cotton swabs. A thin layer of radiopaque glass ionomer restorative cement (GC America Inc., USA) was applied to the skull and around the base of the reinforcing screws to increase adhesion strength between the skull and head holder cap. After allowing the radiopaque cement to set, two horizontal stainless steel head restraint bars were stereotaxically positioned directly above the exposed dorsal region of the skull (5 mm anterior to bregma and 3 mm posterior to lamda). Bars were positioned approximately 5 mm above the dorsal surface of the skull and acrylic dental cement (Stoelting, USA) was applied between the head restraint bars and the skull. After implantation of screws and head restraint bars, surrounding areas of the skull were disinfected using povidone-iodine and antibiotic ointment (Sigma-Aldrich, USA) was applied around the site of incision. Animals were given 7 days to recover with food and water available ad libitum.

Experimental setup

All behavioral training was conducted using a custom head-restraint system (Old School Industries, CO, USA) in a red acrylic box. A reward-delivery nozzle, lever, sensor systems for detecting licking movements and lever-presses, and a speaker were placed around the head-restraint stage. All computer software used for the experiments was customized using LabVIEW (National Instruments, USA) and a digital input/output interface (USB-6008, National Instruments, USA) was used for controlling all devices.

Behavioral conditioning procedure

Habituation to training device

After animals recovered from surgery, they were handled for 3–5 days and allowed to explore (20 min/day) the operant conditioning boxes that they were restrained within for the remaining duration of the experiment. The purpose of this was to familiarize the animal with the experimental environment and reduce context-related stress and excitability. Subjects were head restrained on an ergonomically designed custom head-restraint system (Old School Industries, CO, USA) for 60 minutes/day during the habituation period of the training procedure. During this time period, subjects were administered 0.3 M sucrose (Sigma-Aldrich, USA) non-contingently through a reward delivery nozzle positioned near the mouth. Approximately 40 µl sucrose was delivered every 10 seconds (10 s inter-trial interval (ITI)) over the course of the 60 minute habituation session, yielding a total reward delivery of 15 ml/60 minute session.

Acquisition of lever-pressing and lever/tone association

Following successful sucrose consumption under non-contingent reward delivery conditions, subjects were trained to press a right forepaw lever to receive sucrose reinforcement (Figure 1). This stage of operant behavioral training was intended to facilitate the contingent relationship between lever pressing and reward delivery. This phase of training persisted until the subject responded with >1000 lever presses for 2–3 consecutive days. The next phase of training was the Lever/Tone association phase during which subjects learned to lever press in the presence of an audible tone. The two tones randomly presented were either 3000 Hz or 9000 Hz, which corresponded to the same frequencies used in the Go and No-Go constituents of the later stages of the training procedure. This stage of training began with a 5 s tone presentation with 8 µl sucrose delivered for every lever press occurring during the tone (15 s ITI). Subjects were switched to the next stage of training after >70% correct responding during the presence of the tone. The next phase of training was a reduction in the duration of the tone presentation (5 s to 3 s). After achieving >70% correct responding using these parameters, tone duration was finally decreased to 2 s with a single lever press contingent sucrose delivery of 40 µl. The final phase persisted until subjects achieved >80% correct responding for 2 consecutive sessions.

ebe7a630-0ae7-4204-98db-3a29a6d1da20_figure1.gif

Figure 1. Schematic showing the head-fixed Go/No Go task under a No-Go trial and Go trial.

Lever presses, initiation tone, challenge period, reward tone, and sucrose reward delivery components are shown for both trials. The black boxes represent the relative timing of the rat’s response (lever press) in response to the auditory cues. ITI: inter-trial interval.

No-Go acquisition

First, subjects were trained to perform the No-Go constituent of the behavioral task after successfully completing the Lever/Tone association phase (Figure 1). During No-Go trials, the subject was required to initiate a trial by lever pressing in response to 3000-Hz, 2-s tone (initiation tone). Correct trial initiation yielded an 8 µl sucrose delivery. Following the initiation tone response, a variable delay (1–3 s) was followed by a 2-s white-noise presentation (challenge period). This was the specific epoch in which the subject must withhold from lever pressing (No-Go). Following successful suppression of lever pressing during the challenge period, the 2-s 3000-Hz tone was presented again (reward tone). A second lever press was required during the presence of the final reward tone and a larger 40 µl sucrose was delivered in response to this lever press. Following a 15-s ITI, the next initiation tone was presented. If the subject failed to initiate a trial or withhold from lever pressing during the challenge period, the trial was terminated and a 15-s ITI followed. No-Go acquisition training proceeded until the subject responded at >80% correct challenge period responding for two consecutive days before moving to the Go condition training.

Go acquisition

After successful acquisition of the No-Go constituent of the training procedure, subjects began daily 90 minute Go training sessions (Figure 1). In a Go trial, subjects must lever press to initiate a trial during the presence of a 9000-Hz, 2-s initiation tone. 8 µl of sucrose was delivered in response to the lever press. Following initiation of a trial, a 2-s white noise (challenge period) was presented after a variable delay (1–3 s). The subject must lever press (Go) during the challenge period. Lever pressing during the challenge period terminated the white noise and was followed by a 2-s final reward tone (9000 Hz). The larger 40 µl sucrose reward was only delivered if the subject correctly lever pressed during the reward tone. In the early stage of Go training, a white noise signal of indefinite duration was used for the challenge period. As subjects learned the association between the 9000-Hz initiation tone and lever pressing during the challenge period their reaction time decreased and the challenge period duration was decreased to 2 s. Subjects were trained using these parameters until they achieved >80% correct responding during the 2-s challenge period for 2 consecutive days before a randomized within session Go/No Go condition began (data not included).

Results

Subjects rapidly acquired the ability to consume sucrose delivered non-contingently through a reward delivery nozzle. Following acquisition of sucrose consummatory behavior, subjects quickly learned (<3 days) an operant response by lever pressing (Lever) to receive sucrose reinforcement (Figure 2). On average, subjects reached the specified criteria of >1000 presses/90 minute training session in <3 consecutive daily training sessions (n=24). Next, subjects reliably acquired operant lever pressing behavior during the presence of a tone (Lever/Tone association) to receive sucrose reinforcement (Figure 2). The Lever/Tone association stage of training required an average of 8 training sessions to achieve the criteria of >70% correct responding for two consecutive days (n=20). This requisite stage of training was intended to facilitate the association between tone presentation, lever pressing, and concomitant primary reinforcement. No-Go training (No-Go) followed the Lever/Tone association training (Figure 2). The No-Go constituent of the behavioral task required an average of 22 daily training sessions to achieve criteria of >80% correct challenge period responding (n=14). Finally, subjects were trained to perform the Go constituent (Go) of the Go/No-Go task (Figure 2). This component of the task required an average of 14 daily training sessions to achieve a criteria of >80% correct 2 s challenge period responding for 2 consecutive days (n=13). Raster plots of five consecutive representative No-Go trials (Figure 3A) and histograms of lever pressing and licking during the initiation tone, challenge period, and reward tone are shown (Figure 3B). The histograms represent cumulative responding across a 90 minute training session. Subjects lever pressed in response to an initiation tone and received an initial small sucrose reinforcement (8 µl), withheld from lever pressing (No-Go) during the challenge period, and emitted a final lever press during the final reward tone to receive a larger 40 µl sucrose reinforcement. One example of a failed No-Go trial is shown (4th trial on the raster plot). The subject emitted a lever press during the challenge period of the No-Go trial, resulting in a failed trial and no final 40 µl sucrose delivery. Raster plots of five consecutive Go trials (Figure 4A) and histograms of lever pressing and licking during the initiation tone, challenge period, and reward tone are shown (Figure 4B). Subjects initiated a trial by lever pressing during the initiation tone, emitted a lever press during the challenge period (Go), and lever pressed during the final reward tone to receive sucrose reinforcement. One example of a failed Go trial is shown (1st trial on the raster plot). The subject failed to lever press during the challenge period of the Go trial and the final 40 µl sucrose reward was not delivered. Once the specified criteria of >80% correct responding for 2 consecutive days was achieved for No-Go and Go training sessions, subjects (n=10) maintained stable challenge period responding between subsequent training sessions (Figure 5A). Similarly, subjects stably responded during the challenge period within individual No-Go and Go training sessions (Figure 5B). The data set from all rats is presented.

ebe7a630-0ae7-4204-98db-3a29a6d1da20_figure2.gif

Figure 2. The average and standard error of the mean training sessions required to achieve specified criteria for acquisition of each head-fixed training condition of the behavioral task is shown.

Lever: n=24, Lever/Tone: n=20, No-Go: n=14, Go: n=13.

ebe7a630-0ae7-4204-98db-3a29a6d1da20_figure3.gif

Figure 3.

Raster plot of five consecutive No-Go trials (A) and histograms of lever pressing and licking during initiation tone, challenge period, and reward tone (B). Green bar: initiation tone and reward tone, orange bar: challenge period, magenta bar: reward delivery, black: lick, blue: lever press. (Histogram bin size=100 ms).

ebe7a630-0ae7-4204-98db-3a29a6d1da20_figure4.gif

Figure 4.

Raster plot of five consecutive Go trials (A) and histograms of lever pressing and licking during the initiation tone, challenge period, and reward tone (B). Green bar: initiation tone and reward tone, orange bar: challenge period, magenta bar: reward delivery, black: lick, blue: lever press. (Histogram bin size=100 ms). The second peak on the histogram of lever pressing during the challenge period represents lever presses during the final reward tone. Note the emergence of non cued lever presses and licking during the challenge period that represents the idiosyncratic variable initiation during the Go challenge tone and subsequent reward tone delivery under the Go condition.

ebe7a630-0ae7-4204-98db-3a29a6d1da20_figure5.gif

Figure 5.

Stability of the task performance between (A) and within (B) sessions after reaching criteria. A. Session 0 represents the day when the rats (n=10) achieved the criteria (two consecutive training sessions with >80% (dashed horizontal grey line) correct challenge period responding). B. Within session challenge period responding for 200 trials, divided into 20 trial bins, is shown for the fourth training session after achieving criteria. Error bars indicate the standard error of the mean.

Data availability

figshare: Variable delay Go/No-Go task data, http://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.65401910

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 2
VERSION 2 PUBLISHED 10 May 2013
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Dolzani SD, Nakamura S and Cooper DC. A novel variable delay Go/No-Go task to study attention, motivation and working memory in the head-fixed rodent [version 2; peer review: 2 approved]. F1000Research 2014, 2:125 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.2-125.v2)
NOTE: If applicable, it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 2
VERSION 2
PUBLISHED 19 Mar 2014
Revised
Views
29
Cite
Reviewer Report 21 Mar 2014
Jeff Dalley, ICCAM Cambridge, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK 
Approved
VIEWS 29
I confirm that I have read this submission and believe that I have an ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Dalley J. Reviewer Report For: A novel variable delay Go/No-Go task to study attention, motivation and working memory in the head-fixed rodent [version 2; peer review: 2 approved]. F1000Research 2014, 2:125 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.2327.r4196)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 10 May 2013
Views
50
Cite
Reviewer Report 09 Jul 2013
Jeff Dalley, ICCAM Cambridge, Department of Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 50
This is an interesting and well-conducted study on the development and validation of a novel, symmetrically-reinforced Go/No-Go task in rats. The novelty of this procedure presumably rests with head fixing the animals to limit the impact of extraneous behaviour, and ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Dalley J. Reviewer Report For: A novel variable delay Go/No-Go task to study attention, motivation and working memory in the head-fixed rodent [version 2; peer review: 2 approved]. F1000Research 2014, 2:125 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.1165.r1050)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Views
42
Cite
Reviewer Report 03 Jul 2013
Kenichiro Tsutsui, Department of Biology, Tohoku University, Sendai, Japan 
Approved
VIEWS 42
This interesting paper describes the details of a go/no-go task designed for head-restrained rats. A detailed description about the training procedure is obviously beneficial for the biological community since the use of behaving rats is emerging in ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Tsutsui K. Reviewer Report For: A novel variable delay Go/No-Go task to study attention, motivation and working memory in the head-fixed rodent [version 2; peer review: 2 approved]. F1000Research 2014, 2:125 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.1165.r1038)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 2
VERSION 2 PUBLISHED 10 May 2013
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.