Keywords
online learning, format preference, graduate-level, hybrid-course format
online learning, format preference, graduate-level, hybrid-course format
Hybrid learning courses are defined as courses that deliver 30% to 79% of material via an online medium (Allen & Seaman, 2008). Hybrid courses provide convenience and flexibility through online components while also maintaining the traditional face-to-face experience (Cavanagh, 2011). When designed well, they may also accommodate a wider variety of student learning styles. A meta-analysis conducted by the U.S. Department of Education concluded that students taking hybrid or blended instruction courses had improved outcomes compared to either pure face-to-face learning or pure online learning (Means et al., 2009).
There is little research on hybrid course engagement among adult learners at the graduate level. Adult learners are thought to show greater motivation and learning success based on course metrics like final grades, assignment completion and knowledge retention over time (Coogan, 2009). Further, some researchers posit that modern technology may present some challenges to adult learners that differ from those of younger generations (Coogan, 2009). Thus, the age of students could play a role in hybrid course engagement and outcomes. Further, hybrid course learning may increase accountability as students are expected to endorse self-learning and self-discipline (Coogan, 2009; Hu & Hui, 2012).
Much of the literature on the determinants of course format uptake tests for associations with student learning preferences, self-efficacy and engagement (Hu & Hui, 2012; Shukor et al., 2014). The literature on graduate-level hybrid courses rarely mentions the influence of life circumstances on course options in addition to individual characteristics and preferences. Among adult learners, external influences could potentially be strong indicators of course format preferences. These influences may include travel time, home and work responsibilities.
The present study aims to investigate course format preferences among graduate-level students and possible influential factors for such format preferences. We offered an Introductory Biostatistics course resembling a traditional in-person lecture and lab format, but with added options for synchronous online attendance (while class was occurring) and asynchronous online viewing (at a later time). Students were allowed to choose how to attend or view any class, and we studied their preferences through a post-course survey of habits, life situation, and learning style, and through analysis of YouTube viewing data.
The master's level introductory biostatistics course was offered in a 12-week semester with classes, two exams, and a final project poster presentation, and is a required course for students pursuing a master of public health (MPH). Each class involved one hour of lecture and two hours of laboratory instruction. In-person attendance was mandatory for the first class, exams, and final project poster presentation; for all other sessions students were given the choice of attendance in-person, online during class (synchronous), or online after class ended (asynchronous). The synchronous offering was provided using Google’s “Hangouts on Air,” with a live screen share and audio broadcast viewable through YouTube®. The “Q&A Module” allowed viewers to type questions that appeared on the instructor’s screen during lecture. These questions were answered verbally as were questions raised by in-person attendees, and became a clickable index of the lecture. Recordings became available immediately afterwards on the course’s YouTube channel (https://www.youtube.com/user/ph750spring2014).
This format required that the computer used to present material in class be connected to internet and have a working microphone. In the absence of a dual-screen setup, the “Q&A” module was shown side-by-side with a Microsoft PowerPoint® window used to show lecture slides. A “Lavalier” microphone was used to improve sound quality, but an ordinary webcam microphone would suffice.
A total of 47 introductory biostatistics students were given the opportunity to participate in a survey at the end of the semester. An additional 58 introductory epidemiology students whose course followed a more typical hybrid format of alternating in-person and online sessions were also surveyed for comparison. This study was reviewed by the Hunter College (City University of New York) Institutional Review Board (590445-1) and determined exempt according to federal regulations, under 45 CFR 46.101(b). Informed electronic consent was obtained as part of an anonymous online questionnaire. Study participants were given the option and incentive of entering a draw for a $50 Amazon gift certificate upon completion of the questionnaire.
The end of course survey included demographic characteristics such as age, gender, race/ethnicity and travel time to campus. Participants were asked to choose which format was their preferred way to attend lectures. They were subsequently asked an open-ended question, “Why did you prefer this format?” Responses that pertained directly to the question asked were analyzed and assigned primary and secondary topics based on recurring and prevalent themes within the total set of responses. The keyword topics used to categorize responses were as follows: 1) interactivity, 2) convenience, 3) avoiding commute and 4) learning preference. The complete survey and summarized responses are provided in the Supplementary material and Data availability sections, respectively.
Data were collected via the Google Forms® encrypted website and is noted under the Data availability section.
Viewership among other metrics are automatically documented by the YouTube® website and can be accessed using the analytics dashboard. The dashboard can be found under “analytics” in the “creator studio” section of the Hunter College PH750 Spring 2014 YouTube® account. Data within the time frame of February 1, 2014 to March 4, 2014 were extracted from this dashboard and saved as a comma separated values file. See the Data availability section.
Statistical power was estimated using the pwr.2p2n.test function from the R pwr package. For a sample size of N=26 and a two-proportion test, we estimated 90% power to detect an effect size of between 1.3 and 1.6 for balanced groups and for groups with a 4:1 prevalence ratio respectively.
The survey data were downloaded from the Google Forms® encrypted website and is noted under the Data availability section. Bivariable relationships were only investigated among biostatistics students due to a low response rate from the introductory epidemiology course (19% or 11/58 of these students responded to the survey). Two respondents who were registered for both courses were considered as biostatistics students.
Associations between continuous variables like age and travel time to class and course format preference were analyzed by non-parametric Kruskal-Wallis analysis of variance. Associations between categorical variables and course format preference were assessed by Fisher’s Exact Test. YouTube longitudinal viewing data were visualized as stacked line plots.
Data cleaning and all analyses and plotting were performed using R version 3.1.1 (R Core Team, 2014). Code and data to reproduce all results in this manuscript are provided at https://github.com/LiNk-NY/H-HybridCE.
A total of 26 introductory biostatistics students out of 47 or 55% participated in the anonymous survey. Demographic characteristics of the biostatistics students are shown in Table 1. The majority of these respondents were female (72%), with an average age of 33 years (SD = 8). The median age of the sample was 30 years with a range between 22 and 55 years of age. Of those who answered the survey, 46% of students identified as Non-Hispanic white, 27% as Non-Hispanic black and 15% as Hispanic. The average travel time to the Hunter College Silberman School of Social Work building was 97 minutes (SD = 63 minutes). The median commute time was 90 minutes with a range from 2 to 240 minutes.
Variables - n (%) | Biostatistics students n = 26 |
---|---|
Age (yrs) M (SD) | 32.7 (8.1) |
Median (Range) | 30.5 (22–55) |
Gender | |
Female | 18 (72) |
Male | 7 (28) |
Race-Ethnicity | |
Non-Hispanic White | 12 (46) |
Non-Hispanic Black | 7 (27) |
Hispanic | 4 (15) |
Other | 3 (11) |
Travel time (min) M (SD) | 96.9 (63.4) |
Median (Range) | 90 (2–240) |
Course format preference | |
In person | 12 (46) |
Synchronous | 9 (35) |
Asynchronous | 5 (19) |
Re-take course format preference | |
8 online, 4 in-person | 0 (0) |
In-person only | 3 (11) |
Online only | 3 (11) |
Option of attending each class in-person or online | 18 (69) |
Other | 2 (8) |
Preference reasons endorseda | |
Interactivity | 9 (35) |
Convenience | 7 (27) |
Avoiding Commute | 6 (23) |
Learning Preference | 10 (38) |
Table 1 also shows self-reported course format preferences and preference reasons. Forty-six percent of respondents preferred the in-person format; thirty-five percent preferred the synchronous course format, and nineteen percent preferred the asynchronous online format. Students were asked, “If you were to take this class again next semester, which format would you choose?” The majority of students (69%) would prefer having the option of both for each class, in-person or online, as was done in this class. “Learning preference” was the most endorsed reason with 10 mentions (making up 38% of respondents), followed by “interactivity” with 9 mentions (35%), “convenience” with 7 (27%), and “avoiding commute” with 6 or 23% of biostatistics students.
A qualitative analysis of the long response items in the survey extracted primary and secondary reasons for course preferences. Primary and secondary reasons were determined according to order of appearance. Secondary reasons were only documented for those with more than one reason in the same long response item. These preferences were categorized into four salient topics: 1) interactivity, 2) convenience, 3) avoiding commute, and 4) learning preference. Interactivity was defined as the advantage to be able to ask questions and interact with either the professor or classmates during in-person or online lectures. For example, responses similar to “I prefer class in-person because it allows for interaction with classmates and for me to ask questions in real time” were coded as “interactivity”. The “convenience” topic was determined in any comments where students indicated learning with less external effort or difficulty. Comments similar to, “I liked to be able to pause the lectures and rewind/repeat statements to better understand difficult topics,” were categorized as “convenience” reasons. The “avoiding commute” topic was frequent enough to warrant its own category. Responses that indicated saving time, for example, “I did not have to commute to Harlem,” were coded as “avoiding commute”. Learning preference is indicated when students’ comments explain cognitive advantages to their course preferences.
Table 2 shows relationships between reported course format preference and a number of demographic characteristics along with primary preference reasons. Only the given qualitative reasons for preference were found to be significantly related to course format preference (p <.001, Fisher’s Exact Test). Choice of traditional in-person course format was strongly associated with preference for “interactivity” (58% of those who prefer the in-person format) and with ingrained “learning preference” (50% of those who prefer the in-person format). Conversely, among those who favored the synchronous online course format, “avoiding the commute” was the more frequent reason for such choice. Those who preferred the asynchronous online course format most commonly reported “convenience” as the reason for their choice followed by “learning preference”.
Course Format Preference n (%) | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|
Variable | In-person n = 12 | Synchronous n = 5 | Asynchronous n = 9 | p-value |
Age (yrs) M(SD)a | 34.3 (10.1) | 32.5 (9.3) | 31.0 (4.5) | 0.959 |
Median (Range)b | 30 (25–55) | 32.5 (22–43) | 32 (25–37) | 1.000 |
Travel time (min) M(SD)a | 81.4 (66.2) | 96.5 (59.7) | 117.8 (62.6) | 0.497 |
Median (Range)b | 82.5 (2–210) | 120 (50–240) | 120 (2.5–160) | 0.523 |
Gender | 0.440 | |||
Female | 7 (39) | 3 (17) | 8 (44) | |
Male | 4 (57) | 2 (29) | 1 (14) | |
Race-Ethnicity | 0.425 | |||
Non-Hispanic White | 7 (58) | 1 (8) | 4 (33) | |
Non-Hispanic Black | 1 (14) | 3 (43) | 3 (43) | |
Hispanic | 2 (50) | 1 (25) | 1 (25) | |
Other | 2 (67) | 0 (0) | 1 (33) | |
Reasons for Preferencec | <.001 | |||
Interactivity | 7 (58) | 2 (40) | 0 (0) | |
Convenience | 0 (0) | 2 (40) | 5 (55) | |
Avoiding Commute | 0 (0) | 4 (80) | 2 (22) | |
Learning Preference | 6 (50) | 0 (0) | 4 (44) | |
STEM degree status | 0.130 | |||
Yes | 4 (40) | 4 (40) | 2 (20) | |
No | 8 (50) | 1 (6) | 7 (44) | |
Hybrid-course history | 0.239 | |||
Zero | 5 (50) | 3 (30) | 2 (20) | |
One | 2 (67) | 1 (33) | 0 (0) | |
Two or more | 5 (38) | 1 (8) | 7 (54) | |
Responsible for care of children? | 0.093 | |||
Yes | 1 (20) | 0 (0) | 4 (80) | |
No | 11 (52) | 5 (24) | 5 (24) | |
Employment status | 0.083 | |||
Part-time | 0 (0) | 2 (50) | 2 (50) | |
Full-time | 9 (47) | 3 (16) | 7 (37) | |
None/Prefer not to answer | 3 (100) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | |
Confidence level in statistics before course | 0.408 | |||
1 Not at all confident | 2 (40) | 0 (0) | 3 (60) | |
2 | 3 (37) | 2 (25) | 3 (37) | |
3 | 6 (60) | 3 (30) | 1 (10) | |
4 | 1 (33) | 0 (0) | 2 (67) | |
5 Very confident | 0 (0) | 0 (0) | 0 (0) |
Note. STEM = Science, Technology, Engineering, and Mathematics. Fisher's exact test p-values are shown for all tests unless otherwise indicated. a. Kruskal-Wallis test p-values. b. Median Fisher's exact test p-values are shown. c. Percentages shown by preference group where multiple preference reasons were possible.
Figure 1 shows both primary and secondary reasons provided in qualitative survey responses, by course format preference. Those who preferred the in-person course format endorsed “interactivity” and “learning preference” more often than those who preferred either the synchronous or the asynchronous formats. Those who preferred the asynchronous format tended to mention “convenience” and “learning preference” as reasons for such choice. Among those who preferred the synchronous format, avoiding the commute was the dominant reason for format preference.
Combined primary and secondary self-reported reasons for course format preference were extracted from qualitative topic analysis of an open-ended survey item. Among students who preferred the in-person course format, interactivity was mentioned 7 times in response to the open-ended question, “Why did you prefer this format?” Those who preferred the asynchronous online format mentioned its convenience 5 times in response to the open-ended question. Students who preferred the synchronous online format were likely to mention avoiding the commute as a reason for such preference.
Association between life circumstances and course format preference was nearly significant, with students responsible for the care of children and students working full-time more likely than others to prefer asynchronous viewing (p=.09 and p=.08 respectively, Fisher’s Exact Test). Age, travel time to campus, gender, race-ethnicity, having a STEM undergraduate degree (Science, Technology, Engineering, Mathematics), having previously taken a hybrid-format course, and self-reported confidence level with statistics were not associated with course format preference.
YouTube view frequency patterns of lecture videos were recorded and visualized as stacked line plots. Figure 2 shows the number of views per online video lecture up until the first exam, where the reference line indicates the date of the first exam. The viewing patterns show a high number of views on the date the lecture video is uploaded and an increased number of views for all lecture videos right before the occurrence of the exam. The small increase in viewing between two holidays, Lincoln’s birthday (February 12) and President’s day (February 17) relative to other non-class days shows that some students used those days off to watch lecture videos. This viewing pattern was consistent during the rest of the course and before the second exam.
Shaded peaks reflect increased live-online lecture viewership. Stacked areas show cumulative views per lecture video. An increase in online views for all lectures is observed just before the first exam on March 3rd (indicated by a dashed reference line). Smaller stacked shaded areas reflect asynchronous online viewership of lecture videos with a marked increase in views just before the exam. This pattern of most online attendance occurring during the lecture, followed by low but steady rates of asynchronous viewing and a viewing peak immediately before exam was observed among the remaining video lectures and before the second exam.
We propose a flexible hybrid course format that allows students to choose between traditional on-campus format, interactive online attendance, or viewing lectures at a later time. This course was offered to mature graduate students (average age over 30), mostly working, spread across a large area within and around New York City with an average commuting time to and from campus of over 90 minutes. This format enabled some students to save long commutes, to attend class while traveling, and to re-watch lectures to solidify difficult concepts, while recognizing that a substantial proportion still prefer to attend class in person. Students in this study were evenly split between preference for in-person or online course format, an observation often qualitatively noted by our faculty, and corresponding closely to the actual proportion of students who attended each class on campus. Among students who preferred to attend online, most preferred to attend while class was occurring, largely for reasons of interactivity and ingrained learning preferences. Having to care for children, employment status, and travel time to campus may also contribute to format preference, but these associations were weaker and not statistically significant likely due to a small sample size and thus, insufficient statistical power (see Statistical Power section of Methods).
Our survey indicated that learning preferences were a stronger indicator of course format preference than commute time, job or family status. As an example, one student travelled 3.5 hours round trip to every class because it was “easier to pay attention and take notes without distractions such as email, Facebook etc”. In an environment of increasing commitment by colleges to online education and to expanding reach and enrollment, it is important to recognize the importance of the on-campus, in-person learning experience to many students. This flexible hybrid course format recognizes the diversity of personal circumstances and learning preferences and allows effective remote learning without sacrificing the on-campus experience. It requires only modest adaptation of a traditional in-person lecture format, inexpensive technology, and software that is free of cost. It is an example of how learning technology can be used to expand available learning options and make higher education possible for a greater number of students, without restricting the choice of students who still prefer to sit face-to-face with the teacher and other students.
Data and code to reproduce the results of this manuscript: https://github.com/LiNk-NY/H-HybridCE.
Available data files are:
• Demographics: YouTubeDemographics.csv
• YouTube Data: Figure 2 YouTubeData.csv
• Survey Data: PH750-2SurveyData.xlsx
Archived data and code as at the time of publication: http://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.159623 (Ramos, 2016)
Written informed consent for publication of the participant’s responses was obtained from the participant.
LW, HJ, MR, and MG conceived the study. LW and HJ designed the survey. LW and HJ carried out the data collection. MR and LW prepared the first draft of the manuscript. HJ and MG contributed to the study design. MR prepared the manuscript and analyses. All authors were involved in the revision of the draft manuscript and have agreed to the final content.
Views | Downloads | |
---|---|---|
F1000Research | - | - |
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
|
- | - |
Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?
Yes
Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?
Partly
Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?
Yes
If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?
Partly
Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?
Yes
Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?
Partly
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?
Yes
Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?
Partly
Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?
Yes
If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?
Partly
Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?
Yes
Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?
Partly
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: Educational technology
References
1. Krippendorff K: Content analysis: An introduction to its methodology. Sage. 2004.Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Invited Reviewers | |||
---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | |
Version 1 02 Nov 16 |
read | read | read |
Provide sufficient details of any financial or non-financial competing interests to enable users to assess whether your comments might lead a reasonable person to question your impartiality. Consider the following examples, but note that this is not an exhaustive list:
Sign up for content alerts and receive a weekly or monthly email with all newly published articles
Already registered? Sign in
The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.
You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.
You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.
If your email address is registered with us, we will email you instructions to reset your password.
If you think you should have received this email but it has not arrived, please check your spam filters and/or contact for further assistance.
Comments on this article Comments (0)