ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Research Article

Dose response and working memory limit in an eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing prospective case series

[version 1; peer review: 1 approved with reservations, 1 not approved]
PUBLISHED 17 Sep 2018
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

Abstract

Background: Eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR) is a psychological therapy for post-traumatic stress disorder, or any disorder where the patient reports distressing imagery. We report here a prospective case series to test the prediction that the average number of distress images tends to seven.
Methods: Patients in a sexual health clinic were offered EMDR treatment. In total, 130 were entered and 50 completed treatment. All distressing images to all bad life events and anxieties reported were treated. Images that caused high distress or stopped progressing were usually decomposed until progress resumed.
Results: The median number of images per patient was seven. This required three treatment sessions in a total of five appointments, on average. This result was replicated twice in separate retrospective case series.
Conclusion: We propose that EMDR works by unloading an overloaded memory buffer. If this bandwidth is liberated by treatment, this permits the cognitive and emotional change observed in EMDR treatment. The tendency to seven may signal involvement of the working memory limit. This approach enables clinical decision making and gives common ground with other psychotherapy methods.

Keywords

Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing, Dose dependency, Working memory, Clinical decisions, Seven

Introduction

Eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing (EMDR) is a psychological therapy, originally for post traumatic distress disorder (PTSD), but generalizing to any patient who reports distressing imagery1,2. The image is specified with negative cognitions and distressing physical sensations. The patient is then taken through a procedure based on series of sets of 25 eye movements with the guidance of the therapist. The patient will report the image is fading or changing in some way, the negative cognitions improving and the distress reducing. The cognitions and distress improve without necessarily being addressed by direct verbal methods. Other methods of sensory stimulation have the same effect. There is no explanation of how this procedure works. An explanation is required to inform clinical judgements and to permit the design of more accurate trials.

On the basis of clinical experience, we hypothesize that EMDR is superior to cognitive behavioural therapy (CBT) because it is faster. If a treatment can be accelerated with equal benefit, then more patients could be treated within a given budget. One premise of a controlled trial is that both control and experimental group receive equivalent treatment, except for the variable being investigated. In psychological trials, this requires that both groups should receive an equal treatment time. The published trials do not always balance the number of sessions or treatment time. Consider a trial in which EMDR is compared to CBT over ten weekly appointments of one hour. CBT includes homework, such as listening to audio files of the trauma event, or real life exposure when practical. Assume this homework takes one hour per day, happens each of the six days the patient does not attend an appointment and is performed with perfect compliance. Therefore, EMDR clients are getting 10 hours of treatment, while CBT clients are getting 70 hours. If the result shows equal results for each therapy, then we should conclude that EMDR is seven times faster than CBT and avoids the problem of homework compliance. Homework compliance is associated with improvement in CBT for PTSD3. Some trials address this problem4 and some do not (e.g 5). For a recent review see 6.

To prove EMDR is faster than CBT, patients in a controlled trial should receive an equal number of sessions. Since we could not do a trial in our circumstances, we investigated if the number of EMDR sessions required is limited by the number of distress images. In our clinic, eye movement therapy has been used since 1991 and was based on Shapiro’s original reports1,2,7 Clinical experience showed that EMDR treatment required treatment of around seven images. We hypothesised that the average number of distress images in an EMDR series would be attracted to seven, plus or minus two, images. A retrospective investigation of 400 case files discovered an average of 5.5 distress images8. To investigate this, we counted the distress images in a prospective series and collected demographic and evaluation data.

Methods

Participants

This study was approved by the Plymouth Local Ethics Committee (number, 1743). All patients gave written informed consent to enter the study. The original analysis plan, using non parametric, required 50 patients to complete. However, this plan was superseded by the analysis described below. The patients were recruited between September 2001 and March 2007.

Patients in the Genitourinary Medicine clinic (GUM) were offered EMDR whenever distressing images were reported. No a priori judgements of single trauma event, multiple events or complexity of case were made. Patients were recruited according to the following inclusion criteria:

  • 1. They reported a defined traumatic event or history;

  • 2. English was their first language;

  • 3. They were not receiving any concurrent psychological therapy or counselling. If they were, they were willing to suspend this during EMDR treatment;

  • 4. They were not suffering any concurrent bad life event or illness;

Patients were disqualified from the study under the following circumstances:

  • 1. They suffered a bad life event during treatment. For example, a serious illness suffered by themselves or family member;

  • 2. Any increase in medication. If medication levels were stable, or decreasing, they were retained;

  • 3. They requested to leave.

A disqualifying event was any active event. For example, a patient with stable diabetes was entered, but a patient who was diagnosed with diabetes the week after entry was removed from the study. Removed patients continued with EMDR or other treatment or help as required. Most had a history of sexual health problems that bought them into GUM in the first place. Some patients were the marital or sexual partners of GUM patients, but without primary sexual health concerns. The following information was collected for each patient: sex, age, marital status, employment status, initially presented trauma, how long since that trauma, any significant mental health or addiction issues, presence of panics, total contact hours and number of EMDR treatment sessions.

Measures

EMDR treatment reduces clarity and perception of distress images and consequently the distress score reduces.

We recorded the number of distress images reduced to a low distress score that remained stable on retesting in subsequent treatment sessions (designated “F”). Distress was measured with the Subjective Units of Disturbance score from zero to ten and the end point was taken as zero, one or two. The Foa PTSD questionnaire was used to measure initial severity9.

Treatment progress was measured with three questionnaires: the Impact of Events Revised, (IOE-R), the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12), and the Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI). The Impact of Events revised version was used, which matches the later definition of PTSD10,11. A drawback of the IOE-R is that it only refers to one bad event. We intended to deal with all reported events. Patients who reported more than one were asked to consider the initial event. The GHQ-12 is a psychiatric screen12. The PTCI measures cognitive symptoms and was chosen to exclude imagery or physiological arousal13.

Therapeutic procedure

All patients received EMDR from the clinical psychologist (AH). This psychologist had 10 years’ experience with EMDR before the study began and was level one and level two trained. EMDR, as used here, was based on the original protocol1 and resembles the “eye movement desensitization” protocol that can be found in Chapter 9 of the third edition of Shapiro’s manual7. However, it does not include all eight stages later described by Shapiro, elsewhere in this book.

Body scan, re-evaluation, debriefing and closure were employed when required. Three parts of the eight-phase EMDR procedure were omitted. The safe place procedure and the “cognitive interleave” were never required. If distress stopped progress, the image was reassessed or decomposed to smaller components. At a certain point, the treatment would start reducing distress again. If the patient was distressed at the end of the session, a brief relaxation was taught. The patients reported cognitive changes during the procedure and the PTCI was intended to measure this. The “positive cognitions” procedure was not used. Here, patients report what positive belief they would like concerning themselves and rate it on a one to seven validity scale. This is then used for “installation” of positive cognitions. The therapist dropped this because switching from discussing the bad event to discussing what the patient might think in an ideal world was confusing and distressing for the patient14. It became redundant since patients would report positive thoughts when treatment continued through the series of distress images.

Treatment sessions began by revisiting relevant issues. All distressing images collected or treated in previous sessions were reassessed. The EMDR was then started. The starting rule was expressed as follows: “We will start on the highest scoring image, because that probably makes the procedure shorter in the long run. If you are not ready to start there, please choose somewhere else to start”. The patient was asked to attend to the chosen image and nod when ready. He or she was then taken through sets of 25 eye movements. A stick 70 cm long, with a coin at one end as a visual target was used to guide the eye movements. If the patient reported a problem with eye movements another method of sensory stimulation was used. This was auditory or tactile stimulation using the (www.neurotekcorp.com). This change was judged as a clinical decision and no attempt made to balance it between patients. After each set, the patient was reassessed. When a new image was reported, it was treated or recorded for later treatment.

If the patient became distressed, treatment was continued until the distress reduced. If the distress did not reduce after four sets of eye movements the image was changed in some way. The first move was to enquire if there was a more distressing image, negative thought, emotion or physical sensation or pain. Another option was to move to an alternative image that did reduce and return to the difficult image later. The best option was to decompose the target image in some way. One method was to reduce that part of the timeline the patient was focusing on. For example, the event would be divided up into smaller stages of the sequence. The second was to divide up into sensory modalities. For example, a sexual assault could be divided up into the physical pain images, the sound of the assailant’s voice and the feeling of the weight of their body. Each sensory image was treated individually and included in the “F” count. This course of action generally resulted in effective treatment.

All reported distress images were treated. This included any anxiety or distress trigger that could be elicited as an image and scored, not just the initial presented event. EMDR targets also included images of future events (“flashforwards”). Other images were imagined or symbolic representations of events where the patient had been absent or unconscious. Some images were general (semantic) memories, not episodic. For example, a repeated act of child abuse would be reported as one image, not individual episodes with time tags. Such semantic images might decompose to episodes with treatment, but could be treated without this happening. One strategy to use such semantic memories for desensitization was to ask the patient to imagine the face of the assailant or abuser, then describe and score bad emotions.

Research procedure

The patient was briefed on the procedure. Any problems or limitations of EMDR specific to that patient were discussed. The patient was informed of the seven image average to achieve informed consent, but it was emphasized that this was an average. The patient could report more or less than seven. It was made clear that treatment would continue until all reported distressing images, of whatever origin, were treated. The initial assessment explicitly included listing all trauma life events and distress triggers. The patient gave signed consent to the project as required by the local ethical committee.

Each patient received the IoE-R, GHQ12 and PTCI questionnaires on each treatment appointment. On the first treatment appointment, the Foa questionnaire was also given. Questionnaires were completed on arrival, before the appointment began.

In the first session, the EMDR procedure began at an agreed starting point. In the second and subsequent appointments, the patient was reassessed for each image collected in previous sessions. The patient visualised the image, reported if that was impossible, difficult or easy, reported the distress score and then anything else relevant. Treatment then continued, as described above. Treatment sessions continued until all images were desensitised to a distress score of zero, one or two. The number of images desensitized was recorded on each occasion (“F”). The value of F recorded in the first treatment session was zero.

It was not expected that EMDR treatment would be sufficient for all the needs of all the patients or that all patients would complete treatment. Patients in this clinic have a high drop-out rate from medical treatment. We investigated if there were any systematic differences between completers and dropouts, using the data collected. A record was kept of all patient outcomes or destinations.

Postal follow-up

Six months after treatment ended, all 50 patients who completed treatment were contacted by mail. The letter contained a list of recorded distress images and the three questionnaires. The patient was requested to score all items and return by stamped addressed envelope. In total 31/50 scores were returned.

Two informal replications

Another retrospective count of distress images was included in an audit of appointment attendance. This was a “file drawer” sample, to replicate this result. In total 200 cases were audited from GUM, of which 167 (83%) were EMDR cases. These cases were collected between November 2010 and June 2012.

A second retrospective series was collected at GUM during a period when it appeared that some severe cases were showing high numbers of images. The idea was to test the resilience of the hypothesis that the average would tend to seven. These cases were collected between November 2015 and July 2017.

These two series were taken from routine audit databases for which ethical permission is not required by local rules. The three case series reported were all separate, with no overlap of cases.

Data analysis

Data was analysed using R version 3.4.1. R code used in the analysis can be found in Dataset 115.

Dataset 1. R program files, requiring R.app or RStudio.app (on Mac) or equivalent to run (cran.r-project.org).
The scripts were run in R version 3.4.1 (2017-06-30). The first three (GHQ12WilcoxonOnly.R, ImpactESRWilcoxonOnly.R, PTCIWilcoxonOnly.R) calculate the scores shown in Table 2Table 4. The last (RunningMeans&PlotFig1to4.R) takes the "F" value files and calculates the bootstrapping values as described above and plots Figure 1Figure 4.

Results

Patients

To acquire 50 patients who finished treatment, it was necessary to recruit 130 patients, giving a fail to complete number of 80 (62%). There were 104 females and 26 males. The average age was 33 years. A total of 85 (65%) were employed, 25 unemployed, 14 students and 6 retired; 67 (51%) were single, 54 (41%) were married or co-habiting, the 9 remaining were divorced. They were all white British, except one Irish female and one white South African male. These patients were entered into the study between September 2001 and March 2007.

The initially reported trauma event occurred at a mean of 12 years previously. Forty (30%) patients reported medical problems, including 3 stable diabetics. Thirty-nine (30%) reported another mental health or addiction problem, of which 32 reported depression and 12 reported panics; Twenty-four required medication for that problem. Initially presented trauma events, by sex, are shown in Table 1 for all 130 patients. Most patients were sexually assaulted females. No distinction was made between different types of childhood abuse. Sexual misadventure means any such trouble that was not sexual assault. See Dataset 217.

Table 1. Initially presented trauma of eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing patients distributed by sex (n=130).

EventMaleFemale
Rape746
Assault22
Child Abuse930
Bereavement15
Combat20
Sexual misadventure22
Marital abuse17
School bullying10
Medical misadventure09
Occupational Hazard11
Accident02
TOTAL26104
Subject numberSEXAGEMarried?EMPLOYED?PRESENTED TRAUMAHOW LONG AGOMEDICAL textMENTAL HEALTH or ADDICTIONPANICSDEPRESSIONMEDS?Total EMD sessionsTotal Contact HoursFinal or last F valueFoa score
1F25SingleEmployedRape1.5YESNONONONO25814
2M26SingleEmployedRape5NONONONONO7771632
3F25Married/CoHEmployedSexulaMisadventure9NONONONONO2448
4F57Married/CoHEmployedChildAbuse50NONONONONO36526
5F19Married/CoHEmployedRape1YESNONONONO23828
6F30SingleEmployedMedicalMisadventure2NOYESNODepressionYES24734
7F36Married/CoHNoJobRape6NOYESNODepressionYES34941
8F44Married/CoHNoJobChildAbuse35NOYESYESDepressionNO15212251
9F37Married/CoHEmployedChildAbuse24NOYESNODepressionYES1228
10F19Married/CoHNoJobRape0.25NOYESYESDepressionYES671447
11F21Married/CoHEmployedChildAbuse12NOYESNODepressionNO461020
12F33SingleEmployedRape0.25NOYESNODepressionNO53727
13M52Married/CoHNoJobChildAbuse42YESYESNODepressionYES582136
14F19Married/CoHEmployedChildAbuse13YESNONONONO1
15F34DivorcedNoJobRape2YESNONONONO1246
16M30Married/CoHEmployedSchoolBullying15NOYESNONONO696
17M22SingleStudentChildAbuse15NONONONONO1
18F20SingleStudentChildAbuse15YESNONONONO1
19F25SingleEmployedRape0.5NONONONOYES68938
20F34SingleNoJobRapeRape27YESYESNODeppresionYES7101747
21F31Married/CoHEmployedRape14NOYESNONOYES35748
22F40SingleEmployedChildAbuse30NONONONONO1233
23M21SingleEmployedChildAbuse7YESYESYESDepressionYES810042
24F47SingleEmployedChildAbuse37NONONONONO35426
25F27Married/CoHEmployedRape2NONOYESNONO23045
26F48SingleEmployedRapeRape2NONONONONO341040
27F48SingleEmployedRape29NONONONONO469
28F33SingleNoJobRape20YESNONONONO1234
29F56SingleRetiredRape013NOYESNODepressionNO12191221
30M17Married/CoHStudentRape17YESNONONONO1
31F51Married/CoHRetiredRape12YESNONONONO46919
32F0Married/CoHEmployedChildAbuse25NONONONONO78926
33M35SingleNoJobRape0.1NOYESYESDepressionNO34931
34F52SingleRetiredChildAbuse46YESYESNODepressionYES34344
35F45SingleEmployedRape20NOYESNODepressionNO13514
36F48Married/CoHEmployedCombat23NONONONONO24831
37M33Married/CoHNoJobChildAbuse20NONOYESNONO1
38F39Married/CoHEmployedRape20NONONONONO351024
39F27SingleNoJobChildAbuse14NOYESNODepressionNO23236
40F32Married/CoHNoJobCombat10YESNONONONO34449
41M24Married/CoHEmployedMedicalMisadventure1NONONONONO1
42F16SingleStudentRape0.1YESNONONONO351041
43F26SingleEmployedChildAbuse10NONONONONO24636
44M29SingleEmployedRape14NOYESNONONO56634
45M29DivorcedEmployedMedicalMisadventure1NONONONONO34416
46F30Married/CoHEmployedRape0.1NONONONONO23444
47M49Married/CoHEmployedMedicalMisadventure20YESNONONONO4608
48F20SingleNoJobAssault20YESYESYESDepressionYES24345
49M38Married/CoHEmployedRape24NONONONONO24738
50M30SingleEmployedChildAbuse21YESNOYESNOYES1
51F41SingleNoJobMaritalAbuse15YESNONONONO24145
52F51SingleEmployedMaritalAbuse20YESNONONONO1
53F43SingleEmployedChildAbuse35NONONONONO1236
54F34SingleEmployedRape1NONONONOYES1444
55F42DivorcedEmployedChildAbuse30NONONONONO34035
56M29Married/CoHNoJobRape0.2YESYESNODepressionYES23045
57F39SingleEmployedRape0.5NONONONONO1229
58F17DivorcedStudentRape1NONONONOYES12
59F30Married/CoHEmployedChildAbuse22NONONONONO1233
60F31Married/CoHEmployedChildAbuse20NONONONONO24643
61F19Married/CoHEmployedRape01YESNONONONO35822
62F54Married/CoHEmployedMedicalMisadventure1YESNONONONO461342
63F19SingleStudentRape1NONONONONO35633
64F38SingleEmployedRape5NONONONONO35749
65F16Married/CoHStudentRape0.1NONONONONO1
66F46DivorcedEmployedRape0.25YESNONONONO1249
67F27Married/CoHEmployedMedicalMisadventure2NONONONONO47617
68F19SingleEmployedRape5NONONONONO13543
69F25SingleEmployedMaritalAbuse2.5NONOYESNONO341031
70F35SingleEmployedChildAbuse23NONONONONO1231
71M22SingleEmployedChildAbuse10NONONONONO45628
72F32Married/CoHEmployedMaritalAbuse1NONONONONO23335
73F32SingleStudentRape18NONONONONO35623
74F37SingleNoJobRape37YESYESNODepressionYES35344
75F21SingleStudentRape2NONONONONO34120
76F58Married/CoHRetiredBereavement14NONONONONO35639
77F31SingleEmployedBereavement4NOYESNODepressionYES138139
78F35SingleEmployedSexulaMisadventure1YESNONONONO1
79M58SingleEmployedRape42NONONONONO23030
80F32Married/CoHEmployedRape3NOYESNODepressionNO24435
81F37Married/CoHEmployedMedicalMisadventure2NONONONONO24739
82F22Married/CoHEmployedRape1YESNONONONO781117
83F28Married/CoHEmployedSexulaMisadventure12NOYESNONONO1228
84F22DivorcedEmployedRape0.25NONONONONO36732
85F0SingleNoJobBereavement15YESNONONOYES23037
86F18SingleEmployedRape0.2NONONONONO24634
87F47Married/CoHEmployedChildAbuse37NONONONONO46710
88F37DivorcedEmployedRape4NOYESNODepressionNO351024
89F19Married/CoHEmployedBereavement5NONONONONO34318
90F20SingleEmployedRape6YESNONONONO1225
91F33Married/CoHEmployedChildAbuse25YESYESNODepressionYES46941
92F31Married/CoHEmployedChildAbuse21NOYESNODepressionNO891226
93F48SingleNoJobRape3NOYESNONONO45841
94F20SingleEmployedRape0.2NONONONONO56537
95F30SingleNoJobChildAbuse20NONONONOYES67435
96F37DivorcedEmployedMaritalAbuse10NONONONONO23018
97F35Married/CoHEmployedAssault0.75NONONONONO1232
98F45Married/CoHEmployedSexulaMisadventure34YESNONONONO24712
99M27SingleNoJobChildAbuse23NONOYESNONO561044
This is a portion of the data; to view all the data, please download the file.
Dataset 2.Demographic data for participants.

The 50 patients who completed treatment required a mean of 3.8 treatment sessions (mode = 3.0). A treatment session was one hour long. The mean number of contact hours required per patient was 5.04 (mode = 4.0). This includes assessment, treatment sessions, follow-up and time required for other matters.

Psychometric test scores

Convincing improvement in score results are required to validate the results below. 50 cases completed treatment, resulting in 49 complete before and after pairs. There were 31 complete follow-up scores, which were paired with the respective after scores. Good score decreases were observed for all three questionnaires, all of which had low probability values. All score changes and statistics are shown on Table 2Table 4. Follow-up gave stable values for the IoE-R scale and PTCI. Note the PTCI decreased without any items concerning imagery. However, the GHQ-12 follow-up scores increased, with statistical significance. Raw data is available in Dataset 318 with analysis programmes in Dataset 115.

Table 2. Questionnaire score changes: Impact of Event Score-Revised.

FirstLastFUZWPC
51.56.0
6.0

7.0
5.9
-0.4
1126
184
5.6 × 10−9
1.00
0.98
0.49

Scores summarized as medians, for First, Last and Follow-up (FU). There were 48 pairs First v. Last and 31 pairs for Last v. Follow-up. Z = Z score; number of standard deviations difference. W = Wilcoxon signed-rank test value. P = Probability median scores are different by chance; values are multiplied by 2, to adjust for multiple comparison, by Bonferroni correction. C = Common language effect size20; this is the probability that any initial score will be different from any subsequent score15.

Table 3. Questionnaire score changes: Post Traumatic Cognitions Inventory.

FirstLastFUZWPC
14761
61

64
5.8
-0.7
1078
197
8.8 × 10−9
0.94
0.91
0.44

Scores summarized as medians, for First, Last and Follow-up (FU). There were 48 pairs First v. Last and 31 pairs for Last v. Follow-up. Z = Z score; number of standard deviations difference. W = Wilcoxon signed-rank test value. P = Probability median scores are different by chance; values are multiplied by 2, to adjust for multiple comparison, by Bonferroni correction. C = Common language effect size20; this is the probability that any initial score will be different from any subsequent score15.

Table 4. Questionnaire score changes: General Health Questionnaire-12.

FirstLastFUZWPC
175.0
5.0

10.0
5.7
-3.3
988.5
59.5
1.7 × 10−8
0.002
0.93
0.31

Scores summarized as medians, for First, Last and Follow-up (FU). There were 48 pairs First v. Last and 31 pairs for Last v. Follow-up. Z = Z score; number of standard deviations difference. W = Wilcoxon signed-rank test value. P = Probability median scores are different by chance; values are multiplied by 2, to adjust for multiple comparison, by Bonferroni correction. C = Common language effect size20; this is the probability that any initial score will be different from any subsequent score15.

Dataset 3.The before, after and follow-up scores for the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12), the Impact of Events scale (IoE-R) and the Post Traumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI).

Average value of F

The hypothesis was that the average number of distress images would approach seven. The frequency distribution of F is shown in Table 5 (Dataset 419). The mean value for the 50 patients is 7.6 images. The median was seven and the mode was seven. Figure 1 shows the case series of the 50 completed cases as a “law of large numbers” graph. This is a plot of progressively calculated running means on the Y axis, ascending the case series on the X axis. This shows that after about 20 cases, the mean number of distress images is between seven and eight, close to the hypothesised value. Since the data are not a random sample, we cannot assume a conventional statistical model to test this result. However, having shown the average tends to seven, we need a way of assessing and testing variation in this value.

Table 5. Distribution of final F values.

F ValueFrequencyPercentCumulative %
112.02.0
3122.0
471418
52422
691840
7112262
851072
94888
104888
112492
132496
221298
2312100

F = number of distressing images, treated per patient. Mean = 7.62; Mode = 7; Median = 7.

Dataset 4."F" values for the initial prospective 50 cases and then the two retrospective series of 167 and 106 cases.
e2b55a7e-e412-4382-8fca-50e010ac8632_figure1.gif

Figure 1. Graph of Running Mean or “Law of Large Numbers” plot, showing mean number of images tending to the final mean value of 7.68.

For 50 cases in prospective series. Dotted lines show 95% percentile bootstrap confidence intervals.

We can investigate this with bootstrapping21, which simulates repeating the study many times to give an empirical distribution for F. In this case, a sample of 50 cases was taken from the data, at random, with replacement of each value, so there is always 50 cases to sample from. The sampling was repeated 1000 times. Each sample of 50 generates a mean value and consequently confidence intervals can be found for true mean of F. An R program was written to achieve this16,22,23. We used percentile confidence intervals, so the 95% boundaries take the value of the 25th lowest and highest means from the 1000 iterations. This program will give slightly different results each run, but example values are 6.68 and 8.90, which accommodate the hypothesised value and suggests that in groups of 50 patients we would expect the average number of distress images to be between six and nine. The bootstrap confidence interval is skewed right, reflecting that a small number of cases with a high number of distress images have a big influence on the mean in this relatively small group size. The bootstrap values and graphs are in Dataset 217 (RunningMeans&PlotFig1to4.R). The intention to treat value of “F” was determined from the 99 cases who completed at least two sessions and recorded at least one F value. This includes the 50 cases discussed above. The mean was 6.5 and the mode and median both 6.0.

Two retrospective replications

The first retrospective series contained 167 cases (Figure 2). The sample mean is 7.17. Using the same R program, 95% percentile boundaries are 6.57 and 7.86. As we would expect with a larger group size, there is less variation in the mean and the bootstrap confidence interval is more symmetric about the sample mean (Dataset 419). The second retrospective series contained 106 cases (Figure 3). It was biased to start with high F values of 25, 20, 16, with a minimum value of 1. The mean was 8.5, with 95% percentile boundaries of 7.5 and 9.5. This shows increase of the mean with some severe cases in the series (Dataset 419). The two retrospective series included complete cases and those who did not complete, but were considered successful before they failed to attend. If all 323 cases reported here were combined, the mean was 7.68 inside boundaries of 7.20 and 8.20 (Figure 4). This estimates the long-term average of distress images for populations similar to that attending the clinic.

e2b55a7e-e412-4382-8fca-50e010ac8632_figure2.gif

Figure 2. First retrospective replication of 167 cases.

Final mean = 7.17.

e2b55a7e-e412-4382-8fca-50e010ac8632_figure3.gif

Figure 3. Second retrospective replication of 106 cases, starting with high values.

Final mean = 8.50.

e2b55a7e-e412-4382-8fca-50e010ac8632_figure4.gif

Figure 4. All 323 cases (50 + 167 + 106 = 323).

Final mean = 7.68.

Destinations

Table 6 shows the destinations of the 130 patients recruited. Nine patients left the project, but continued treatment, after another life event. EMDR was not sufficient for six patients who required further cognitive therapy (from the same therapist). Twelve attended assessment, consented to the trial, then did not attend treatment (DNA). Patients who dropped out of treatment were divided into those who attended at least two sessions and therefore contributed data, and those who attended once. Questionnaire and other data showed improvement in the drop-outs, not discussed further for reasons of space. Six patients decided to leave EMDR treatment for another therapist. The 50 finishers were compared with the 46 who started treatment but did not finish and for whom all data was collected. No significant differences were detected for any collected parameters.

Table 6. Destinations of patients that underwent EMDR therapy.

EMDR, eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing.

DestinationFrequencyPercent
Another traumatic event96.9
Cognitive therapy64.6
Did not attend treatment129.2
Drop-out after 2 sessions2821.5
Drop-out after 1 session1813.8
EMDR finishers5038.5
Medication increase10.8
To another therapist64.6
TOTAL130100

Discussion

The median number of distress images was seven. We defined and counted the unit “distress image” as that which reduces in distress when treated with eye movements. This definition is validated by the reduction in questionnaire scores. EMDR is considered as a titration of eye movements against distress. Distress memory images can be episodic or semantic. The “distress complex” is the collective noun for those distress images reported by one patient. Three qualifications are required.

Only data from one EMDR therapist is reported. This result requires replication with other therapists, following the same procedure. The two retrospective replications gives some mitigation for this issue. We cannot show replication with other therapists, because only one was available, but we can replicate within one therapist, across time. Second, the patients were informed of the expected seven image average. This raises the possibility that the seven is an artefact. We considered it necessary, and ethical, to inform them of this to achieve informed consent. It was emphasized that seven was an average and the patient should report all images, to all bad life events or distress triggers. The good before and after scores support the position that all images were reported and treated. Third, this was an uncontrolled case series, designed to observe the natural history of treatment to test a hypothesis. These limitations are dictated by the circumstances of an acute sexual medicine clinic, where a fast turnover of patients is required and where there is only one psychologist. Legal and practical difficulties limit communication to patients.

Subject to these issues, we can assert that for the typical population attending the clinic, the distress complex tends to contain seven, plus or minus two, distress images. This is demonstrated by the prospective case series that had a median of seven images. The two retrospective series also stabilised in this zone. These results are similar to those in a previous report9. The largest retrospective series (of 167) had a mean of 7.17 (with near-symmetric bootstrap 95% interval of 6.67–7.86). In the second retrospective series starting with more severe cases, the mean rises to 8.5. Combining all 323 cases moves the mean back towards seven, which we can consider a stable value in the long term. This observation may be sensitive to variation in patients and method. This demonstrates that the EMDR system is dose dependant, because sufficient EMDR is required to desensitize the whole distress complex. Any trial that compares eye movement or sensory stimulation methods with CBT must ensure that the amount of treatment time is equal across groups and is sufficient to desensitize the distress complex. Commonly, such patients have collected more than one trauma event. This issue is rarely addressed in published trials. If only the worst reported life event is treated, then other distress triggers may be missed. This might limit longer term results. This conclusion may apply to all psychological therapies.

Similar numbers are reported from similar patients. Such similarities are worth noting, but should be considered with due caution since there are differences of definition and method in each report. First, the Rothbaum et al. study treated one distressing memory, but reported discovering an average of six others in both patient groups5. This is a total of seven. Second, a trial of EMDR treatment reported that patients were treated until they no longer had PTSD. This required treatment of six trauma memories24. Third, Holmes et al.25 counted the number of “hotspots” in a sample of patients in treatment and discovered an average of six, in the context of an average of four intrusive images of different contents.

Observing this attraction towards seven raises the possibility of involvement of the working memory. Seven is identified as the approximate number of information chunks that can be held in the working memory26. If it is assumed that a distress image is a chunk, then perhaps this is a signal from the working memory limit. Working memory capacity is a complex subject, which we cannot simplify here. For reviews see Cowan27, Jonides et al.28, and Richardson29. It might be a safer initial claim that EMDR therapy works by unloading a limited capacity memory buffer, without specifying further. PTSD and similar disorder is caused by overloading this buffer.

A possible model to explain the unloading is “reverse learning”, originally postulated by Crick & Mitcheson30. The speed of the EMDR effect implies a physiological explanation9. The idea of unloading memory buffers enables several points to be made. Firstly, this is a good explanation for the patient, since such overload is what chronic distress disorder feels like. Second, this enables clinical decisions in the treatment session. For example, on occasion, the patient becomes so distressed, or affected in some other way, that the treatment process stalls. This might mean the memory buffer capacity is saturated. Therefore, the EMDR target image should be progressively decomposed into smaller images, as described above, until distress starts to reduce again. At this point both the EMDR responsive image and the available memory processing bandwidth are defined. An analogy is threading a needle. A thread will not go through the eye of the needle if it is too big, so the size of the thread is reduced until it passes through. This liberated bandwidth enables further synergistic progress. We can now explain the cognitive and emotional change caused by the EMDR procedure. As memory bandwidth is liberated, the patient becomes progressively more able to think rationally. Distress is reduced to zero, or that level permitted by circumstances.

Can anything be said about this memory buffer? First, there must be two limited capacity systems involved. The first contains the whole distress complex and averages seven images. However, when treating each image, we presumably have placed that image into a second buffer, which only holds one. These might be working memory and focus of attention, respectively. It is established that eye movements affect working memory3133. If EMDR affects working memory capacity, then this is common ground with other therapeutic methods, such as mindfulness training34, and counting during recall35, where similar claims have been made.

The observation of a limit gives an approach to dealing with serious, or complex, cases. Using the distribution in Table 5, 96% of cases report up to 13 distress images. Therefore, only 4% of EMDR cases go to 14, or above. Such cases are either serious, or the therapist should consider if the limits of EMDR treatment have been reached and for what reason. Reaching image number 14 requires diagnosing between, for example, a child sexual abuse case with panics, in which case continue EMDR, or another issue which may require reconsidering EMDR. When a history of childhood abuse is reported, the fact that seven is average and fourteen is rare, enables patient and therapist to define the task. Patients may ask if they will require other psychotherapeutic help after EMDR. The answer can be given that, if this is required, they will benefit more from other methods once EMDR has unloaded the working memory up to this limit.

Conclusions

Both the observation of the seven images and the speed of treatment are achieved by this minimalist protocol, with decomposition of obstinate flashbacks. This probably accelerates treatment. Only an average of three treatment sessions and five in total were required per patient. It is likely that this version of the EMDR procedure permits the observation of the distress image horizon. With EMDR using the eight stage sequence, difficulties are solved by methods borrowed from cognitive therapy, or evaded by a retreat to the imagined safe place. In our version of eye movement or sensory stimulation therapy, difficulties are usually solved by decomposing the image into smaller elements that can be individually treated. Reducing an unresponsive distress image into smaller units permits us to continue treatment and define the units by their response to treatment. CBT does not permit this, especially if the distress complex is committed to audio or video file for self-exposure. The theory that it is necessary to match the image with some limit of memory processing bandwidth, which shows one sign of working memory, enables an account that facilitates clinical decisions and may improve trials.

Data availability

F1000Research: Dataset 1. R program files16, requiring R.app or RStudio.app (on Mac) or equivalent to run (cran.r-project.org)., 10.5256/f1000research.15648.d21718315

F1000Research: Dataset 2. Demographic data for participants., 10.5256/f1000research.15648.d21718617

F1000Research: Dataset 3. The before, after and follow-up scores for the General Health Questionnaire (GHQ-12), the Impact of Events scale (IoE-R) and the Post Traumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI)., 10.5256/f1000research.15648.d21718418

F1000Research: Dataset 4. "F" values for the initial prospective 50 cases and then the two retrospective series of 167 and 106 cases., 10.5256/f1000research.15648.d21718519

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 17 Sep 2018
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Hassard A, Turner H and Smith K. Dose response and working memory limit in an eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing prospective case series [version 1; peer review: 1 approved with reservations, 1 not approved]. F1000Research 2018, 7:1471 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.15648.1)
NOTE: If applicable, it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 17 Sep 2018
Views
11
Cite
Reviewer Report 02 Mar 2020
Gaëtan Mertens, Department of Clinical Psychology, Utrecht University, Utrecht, The Netherlands;  Department of Medical and Clinical Psychology, Tilburg University, Tilburg, The Netherlands 
Not Approved
VIEWS 11
In this manuscript titled “Dose response and working memory limit in an eye movement desensitization and reprocessing prospective case series” by Hassard and colleagues, the authors investigate the number of distressing images treated in EMDR treatment. The authors predicted that ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Mertens G. Reviewer Report For: Dose response and working memory limit in an eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing prospective case series [version 1; peer review: 1 approved with reservations, 1 not approved]. F1000Research 2018, 7:1471 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.17073.r60129)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 15 Jun 2020
    Alan Hassard, Clinical Psychology Department, Derriford Hospital, Plymouth, PL6 8DH, UK
    15 Jun 2020
    Author Response
    Response to Dr Mertens
    Thank-you for reviewing our report. The critique made has validity. My defense, or at least plea for mitigation, is partly theoretical and partly about the limitations of research in a ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 15 Jun 2020
    Alan Hassard, Clinical Psychology Department, Derriford Hospital, Plymouth, PL6 8DH, UK
    15 Jun 2020
    Author Response
    Response to Dr Mertens
    Thank-you for reviewing our report. The critique made has validity. My defense, or at least plea for mitigation, is partly theoretical and partly about the limitations of research in a ... Continue reading
Views
15
Cite
Reviewer Report 19 Mar 2019
Lonneke I.M. Lenferink, Department of Clinical Psychology and Experimental Psychopathology, Faculty of Behavioral and Social Sciences, University of Groningen, Groningen, The Netherlands 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 15
In this study the authors conducted a prospective case series study to test the hypothesis that on average 7 distressing images need to be treated in EMDR treatment in a sample of patients who experienced various traumatic events. My suggestions ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Lenferink LIM. Reviewer Report For: Dose response and working memory limit in an eye movement desensitisation and reprocessing prospective case series [version 1; peer review: 1 approved with reservations, 1 not approved]. F1000Research 2018, 7:1471 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.17073.r44265)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 16 Jun 2020
    Alan Hassard, Clinical Psychology Department, Derriford Hospital, Plymouth, PL6 8DH, UK
    16 Jun 2020
    Author Response
    Response to Dr Lenferink.
    This is a response to the qualified approval of Dr Lenferink and answer to issues raised. 

    (1) It is advised that more information is included in the abstract. ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 16 Jun 2020
    Alan Hassard, Clinical Psychology Department, Derriford Hospital, Plymouth, PL6 8DH, UK
    16 Jun 2020
    Author Response
    Response to Dr Lenferink.
    This is a response to the qualified approval of Dr Lenferink and answer to issues raised. 

    (1) It is advised that more information is included in the abstract. ... Continue reading

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 17 Sep 2018
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.