ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Research Article

Early storybook reading and childhood development: A cross-sectional study in Iran

[version 1; peer review: 2 approved]
PUBLISHED 29 Mar 2018
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

This article is included in the Developmental Psychology and Cognition gateway.

Abstract

Background: Development is a process that continues from childhood to death, and most developmental changes occur during childhood. UNICEF introduced early storybook-reading (ESR) and storytelling as part of child care indicators. The aim of this study was to investigate the status of book-reading to children and its relationship with early childhood development in Iran.
Methods: This is a descriptive-analytic study conducted in Tehran April-May 2017. In total, 272 mothers of children aged 3-30 months, who were referred to health centers, were selected using a convenience sampling method. Exclusion criteria was scoring below the cutoff point of any developmental domains of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ). ESR was assessed by checklist and child development was assessed by the ASQ. Data were analyzed using SPSS.
Results: The mean number of children’s books owned was 10.23±8.642, and 84.75% had at least 3 books. The average book reading, storytelling and singing duration for children was 10±9.65, 11.48±11.756, and 23.88 ±17.880 min per day, respectively. Average book reading, storytelling, and singing duration was significantly greater in children 18-30 months than <17 months. There was a significant relationship between the number of books and a child's age, mother's age, family income, income satisfaction, father's employment, and parents’ education. The score of communication domain in the ASQ questionnaire was significantly related to the number of books, duration of reading and storytelling, while problem-solving had a significant relationship only with the number of books (p˂0.05). Based on linear regression, child's age, income, and mother's and father's educational level were models for predicting the number of children's books (p=0.0001 for all).
Conclusions: ESR was associated with some developmental domains of communication and problem-solving in the present study. Therefore, creation of ESR culture in Iranian families as an integral part of the life of children is necessary from birth.

Keywords

Book Reading, Child Development, Early Interventions

Introduction

Development is a complex process through which an individual acquires various capabilities to improve performance and have better adaptation to the environment. Although, this process continues from childhood to death, most developmental process occurs during the first few years of life1, during which the neural structure evolves2. Although the opportunity for early childhood interventions is short, it has significant results3, and it is believed that a better beginning of life will lead to a better future4. Therefore, providing the best beginning for children can lead to success in life, employment, higher earnings, active participation in society, increased responsibility, and reduction of crime and some chronic diseases5,6. The prevalence of developmental disorders in Iranian children is between 7 and 22.4%7 and is reported to be 14–20% in other countries811. However, by encouraging mothers and community participation, and timely and appropriate intervention in controlling the risk factors, the ability to promote development, health, well-being and suitability of the child can also be provided1214.

Many social factors and environmental stimuli are effective in health and early childhood development (ECD)12, which include useful books and toys that provide a healthier life in the long run for the individual. Book reading is regarded as one of the most powerful protections for mental growth and development, and psychological and social stimulation of a child at home15,16. UNICEF introduced early storybook reading (ESR) and storytelling as child care indicators16. In addition, the parent-child's common interest and joint attention in reading picture books provides the basis for social and emotional communication during childhood. Reading books to children promotes their development of speech and language, and cognitive and emotional social behavior1719, and provides them with joy20. In addition, the infant learns how to take the books in their hands and try to turn its pages. Hence, it can promote gross and fine motor coordination and also a love for reading21. When book reading is started in early months of life, a longer and more stable effect on child development will be observed22, as reported by Senechal and LeFevre23 who found that parents should begin book reading to infants at the age of 4 months, since these children had better literacy skills at school age. It is believed that ESR promotes the development of children; Murray and Egan24 reported a positive relationship between reading books and the score of cognitive development in 9-month-old infants. Cline and Edwards stated that ESR in addition to enhancing child's development and learning skills, also creates an interest, motivation and habit of regular book reading in the individual in the future22. Therefore, it is good for all family members to participate in book reading to children, and children’s books should be made available for them at home22. Additionally, during reading, a warm and intimate interaction between adults, especially parents and the children is created, and along with playing, can create love and reading infrastructure for motivation in later years22,25. In addition to book reading, storytelling and singing also have similar benefits in children and can be done anywhere or any time22. It is believed that ESR, storytelling and singing to children are a series of activities that help in the development of speech and language, literacy and children's brain development26. The most important ESR barriers are inadequate access to children's book27, and parents' lack of awareness on the positive effects of ESR and inappropriate strategies for promoting children development2729.

In general, few studies have been conducted on the role of children's books in the lives of children under the age of three30. Reading books to children is considered as an important activity in Western culture31, including the BookStart program in the United Kingdom, which over two decades implemented reading books in the early years of life has as an Early Interventions (EI)32. Although there have been numerous studies on ECD in Iran, reading to young children is not considered as one of the environmental stimuli affecting ECD. Accordingly, this study aimed to examine the status of book reading on Iranian children, including children's books, parents' participation in reading, storytelling and singing to children based on ECD in infants and toddlers without any developmental delay during early childhood in Tehran.

Methods

Study design and setting

This is a descriptive-analytic cross-sectional study that aimed to investigate the status of book reading on young children and its relationship with ECD in Tehran from April to May, 2017.

The research population consisted of all mothers with children from 3 to 30 months, who were referred to one of the public health centers in Tehran for various health-related services. For sampling, Tehran was divided into two regions, East and West, and one health center was selected from each of the two regions randomly Ershad and Shahre Ziba public health centers were selected in the East and West of Tehran, respectively. Sampling of mothers was conducted using convenience sampling method. Mothers that were referred to the health center for a routine checkup for her child, child vaccination and/or receiving other health services, were entered in the study.

Participants

Using a sample size formula1 and a 95% confidence level, and also considering the prevalence of developmental disorders, which was considered to be about 22% in Iran in accordance with previous studies7, with an increase of 10% to take account of potential attrition, 290 people were enrolled in the study, from which 272 consented to participate.

The criteria for entry in this study were parents with a child aged 3 to 30 months who, while referred to the health centers, who were willing to cooperate. At first, the aims of the study were explained to the parents once they were referred to the health center. They also provided written informed consent and completed self-report questionnaires in about 10–15 minutes. The mothers of children with developmental delay (based on ASQ) were excluded.

Assessment tools

The assessment tools of this study were the ASQ, demographic questionnaire and a checklist for assessing the ESR status of children (Supplementary File 1). The demographic questionnaire and checklist were prepared by the research team and validated by 7 pediatricians and specialists in child development who worked in University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences of Iran. These tools were piloted by 8 mothers that were referred to the Saadat Abad Health Center. This checklist included the number of children's books, duration of reading, storytelling and singing for children as variables.

The ASQ is used to screen infants and young children for developmental delays during the crucial first 5 years of life. This tool assess the different developmental domains (gross motor, fine motor, communication, problem-solving, and social-personal)33. For each age group, a total of 30 questions (six questions for each developmental domain) were designed and the highest score available for each question is 10; therefore for each developmental domain, the score is 60. The answer to each question included yes, sometimes and never with 10, 5 and 0 points considered for each question, respectively. This questionnaire is a standard tool that has been translated into various countries in Asia, Europe, Africa and the United States34,35, and the sensitivity and specificity of this test are 88% and 82.5%, respectively35,36. The screening of developmental delay was done by comparing the score of each area with a cut-off point37 based on the age of Iranian children. According to a study by Sajedi et al.38 the Cronbach's alpha obtained was 0.79 and, the validity, reliability, and ability of the test for determination of evolutionary deficits were 0.84, 0.94 and 96%, respectively37. As it is possible that parents may rate their children more favorably, and the ASQ is a self-report questionnaire, the mothers were given adequate explanations of each question and answer before completing the questionnaires in order to reduce reporting bias.

Data analysis

Due to the nature of the present study, demographic data, ESR status for children, and ASQ questionnaire scores were standardized in terms of mean and standard deviation. Categorical data were summarised using absolute values (percentage). After determining the data normality of each variable, Chi-square, independent t-test, and correlation coefficients Spearman’s test were used. Considering the fact that the data gathering tools (i.e. the questionnaire) was a self-reported one, despite of all counter measures, having missing data was inevitable. Assessment of the impacts of such missing data, was made by Univariate T-Test. The results revealed that the missing data were completely at random39. In order to predict the situation of ECD, independent variables were included in the linear regression model. Data were analyzed by SPSS software version 22 at a significant level of p<0.05.

Ethical statement

The present research was approved by the University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences (ethics code IR.USWR.REC.1395.77). This study observed ethical standards, and after sampling, in the case of results from the ASQ showing probability of developmental delay for certain children, this was discussed with pediatricians who were in contact with parents.

Results

The number of mothers included in the study was 272. Most of the children (86.86%) were born by cesarean section method, 50.9% were girls, and 21.5% were also cared for by grandparents in addition to their parents. As shown in Table 1, the mean age of mothers and fathers was 31.72 ± 4.450 and 35.5 ± 5.149 years, respectively. 68.4% of mothers were housewives and 5.5% of fathers were unemployed. The average income of families was 31,150,000 Rials, 54.8% of whom were satisfied with their income. 73% of children did not have any siblings.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics of the family according to age group of the infants and toddlers.

Age group (months)
Variables, mean±SD (range)3 to 17 (N=165)18 to 30 (N=107)Total (N=272)
Infants age (months)9.81±4.022 (3-17)23.83±3.886 (18-30) 15.33±70924
Birth weight infant (g)3161±383 (2200-4300)3063±423 (1950-4100)3.122±0.401
Gestational age (weeks)37.31±1.987 (29-41)36.72±2.462 (28-41)37.04±2.168
Mothers age (years)31.55±4.443 (23-42)31.97±4.469 (21-43)31.72±4.450
Fathers age (years)35.09±5.060 (25-49)36.32±5.218 (25-50)35.58±5.149
Mother’s education (N, %)
Diploma
Academic

28 (17.0)
137 (83.0)

24 (22.5)
83 (77.5)

52 (19.1)
220 (80.9)
Father’s education (N, %)
Diploma
Academic

44 (26.6)
121 (73.4)

34 (31.7)
73 (68.3)

78 (28.7)
194 (71.3)
Mother’s job (N, %)
Employed
Housewife
59 (35.8)
106 (64.2)
27 (25.9)
80 (74.1)
86 (31.6)
186 (68.4)
Father’s job (N, %)
Employed
Unemployed

153 (92.7)
12 (7.3)

104 (97.2)
3 (2.8)

257 (94.5)
15 (5.5)
Income (Rial, monthly)30,380,00±1,483,000
(3,000,000-80,000,000)
32,280,000±2,252,800
(4,000,000-180,000,000)
31,150,000±1,830,000
Satisfaction of income (N, %)
Satisfied
Unsatisfied

84 (50.9)
81 (49.1)

65 (60.7)
42 (39.3)

149 (54.8)
123 (45.2)

On average, every child had 10.2±8.642 books. Among the children, 30 (11.20%) did not have any books and 228 (84.75%) had at least 3 books. Only five children (1.83%) had more than 37 books each. The average daily reading time for children was 9.65 ± 10, while for half of them, nobody read 5–15 min to them a day, and 27.6% of them were never read to. The average storytelling time for children was 11.48 ± 11.756 min per day, while 25.7% of parents had never told the children a story. The average duration of singing for children was 23.88 ± 17.880 min per day, but for 11.4% of them, nobody has ever sung.

According to the results in Table 2, the number of children's books owned, duration of ESR, storytelling and singing in children was significantly different between children aged 3 to 17 months and those aged 18–30 months (p=0.0001 for all). There was a significant relationship between the number of children’s book owned with duration of ESR, storytelling and singing (p<0.0001). In addition, there was a significant association between the number of children's book owned and children's age (p<0.0001), mother's age (p=0.021), family income (p=0.009), income satisfaction (p=0.026), father's occupation (p=0.04), and mother's and father's educational level (p<0.0001). ESR duration for children was significantly correlated with the age of the child, mother and father (p<0.0001). Singing duration was significantly correlated with only the mother's educational level (p=0.011).

Table 2. Early Storybook Reading situation of infants and toddlers.

Age group (months)
Variables, mean±SD (range)3 to 17 (N=165)18 to 30 (N=107)Total (N=272)
Amount of children's books owned
No books owned (number of children, %)
8.02±7.713 (0-40)
25 (15.2)
13.57±8.929 (0-45)
5 (4.7)
10.23±8.642 (0-45)
30 (11)
Duration of book reading (min per day)
No book reading (number of children, %)
7.42±8.571 (0-30)
61 (37)
13.04±11.058 (0-60)
14 (13.5)
9.65±10.001 (0-60)
75 (27.6)
Duration of storytelling (min per day)
No storytelling (number of children, %)
9.93±12.395 (0-60)
60 (36.4)
13.88±10.592 (0-60)
10 (9.3)
11.48±11.856 (0-60)
70 (25.7)
Duration of singing (min per day)
No singing (number of children, %)
22.49±18.4996 (0-90)
25 (15.2)
24.76±16.872 (0-60)
6 (5.6)
23.38±17.880 (0-90)
31 (11.4)

The correlation between ESR variables is shown in Table 3. From the viewpoint of development, the scores of communication and problem-solving from the ASQ questionnaire showed a significant relationship with the number of children's books owned (p<0.0001 and p=0.016). Also, there was a significant correlation between the score of communication and duration of ESR and storytelling (p=0.002 and p=0.028). There was no significant relationship between variables and duration of singing (P>0.05 for all variables). Based on linear regression, children's age, income, and mother's and father's educational level can be models for predicting the number of children's books owned (p<0.0001 for all).

Table 3. Correlation between Early Storybook Reading (ESD) and Early Childhood Development (ECD).

ESD
Variables
Domains of ECD, mean±SD N=272)
Number of
children’s
books owned
Duration of
book reading
(min per day)
Duration of
storytelling
(min per day)
Duration of
singing
(min per day)
Communication
(48.70±9.742)
Spearman Correlation
Sig(2-Tailed)
0.279*
0.000
0.136*
0.028
0.191*
0.002
0.169
0.066
Gross Motor skills
(51.76±8.315)
Spearman Correlation
Sig(2-Tailed)
0.113
0.065
0.094
0.126
0.094
0.126
0.096
0.117
Fine Motor skills
(50.02±9.161)
Spearman Correlation
Sig(2-Tailed)
0.096
0.115
0.059
0.345
0.014
0.825
0.119
0.053
Problem-Solving
(52.34±7.010)
Spearman Correlation
Sig(2-Tailed)
0.147*
0.016
0.099
0.110
0.039
0.532
0.089
0.149
Personal-Social
(48.68±9.457)
Spearman Correlation
Sig(2-Tailed)
0.066
0.279
0.040
0.514
0.074
0.231
0.097
0.115

*Correlation is significant at the level 0.05 (2-Tailed).

Dataset 1.ESR and ECD data from the East and West of Tehran.

Discussion

In the present study, the average number of children’s books owned was 10.23±8.642, which increased significantly in infants aged 18 to 30 months as compared to infants less than 17 months old (p=0.0001). In other studies, this number was reported to be 2.6± 3.600 to 7.9± 10.9 for children aged 6 to 18 months, indicating an increase in the number of children's books owned based on the age of the children, and it is believed that during early infancy, playing is considered more important than book reading30. A total of 84.75% of the children had at least 3 children’s books; this was reported to be 97% in Ukraine and 3% in Laos16. According to the results of the current study, more than 68% of the parents had a university education, although in Shahshahani et al.’s40 only 34% of parents had a university education. In the present study, there was a significant relationship between the number of books and the duration of ESR with parental education, while in this case Boyle and colleagues believed that educated mothers used better medical education recommendations41, while fathers with university education can provide a more sustainable financial and social environment for their children21. In addition, mother's level of education plays a facilitating role in the book reading of children and ECD21. According to some similar studies, educated parents also read more books to their children42. However, in the study by Tomopoulos et al.30, maternal education was not associated with ESR. The mean ESR time in the current study for children aged less than 17 months and from 18 to 36 months was 7.42±8.571 and 13.04±11.058 minutes per day, while in the study of 30, the mean for the ages of 6 and 18 months was 2.1±2.300 and 3.5±2.800 days per week, respectively. Although in the present study, father's employment was significantly correlated with the number of children's books owned; there was no significant relationship between father's occupation and ESR, which is similar to result presented in 30.

Given the average number of books in this study, for 61.4% of children aged 0–17 months, book reading was begun at the time of sampling; this statistic increased to 86.5% in children aged 18 to 30, while based on the results of Duursma21, book reading was done daily or weekly for 77% of the children who were 14 and 24-months-old, 52% of children aged 4 to 35 months43 and 50% of children aged 0 to 36 months27. According to Senechal and LeFevre23, the number of picture books in a home has a strong relationship with the children's receptive and expressive language. Therefore, parents should be advised to read books to their children at the earliest opportunity, from birth, because it can be effective in their development5. Meanwhile, in the current study, 28.6% of the children had never been read to, while in other studies, this statistic was 4% in the UK27 and 6–23% in the USA21, 43.

According to the present findings, family income showed a significant relationship with the number of books owned and duration of ESR. In this regard, Karrass and his colleagues44 also found in 2013 that mothers of higher income families had a greater tendency for ESR for their 8-month-old infants. Additionally, according to a UNICEF report16 in 2012, children from poor families had fewer children’s books at home.

In the present study, 62.5% and 90.4% of children aged 0–17 and 18 to 30 months were told stories. Also, 84.6% and 94.24% of children aged 0–17 months and 18 to 30 months were sung to, indicating an increase in these stimuli with the age of the child. In confirmation, other researchers also believe that storytelling and singing to children helps in speech evolution, language and literacy, as well as the evolution of the baby's brain45.

Having a book and reading it at home will provide more opportunities for improving skills46. According to the results of the current study, there was a positive correlation between the number of children's books owned and problem-solving scores. In confirmation of this result, by using ASQ, Murray and Egan’s research also found that reading books to infants had a positive impact on problem-solving and communication issues11. In this regard, Duursma21 also proposed the ability to predict 24-month cognitive skills score by Bayley MDI based on the time spent in reading to a child. Also, loud book reading had a significant effect on the cognitive development of premature infants after two years47. In the present study, there was a positive correlation between the number of children's books owned, the duration of ESR and storytelling for children and the communication score. In relation to the impact of ESR on speech-language development, results of research indicates that children use a rich vocabulary during book reading18,31 and interventions for the development of the children's language, focusing on reading books whenever possible in children's lives are required48. However, in the current research, there was no relationship between ESR and other developmental domain scores, but in another study it was reported that ESR may promote language and social communication skills49.

Limitations

In this study, the important environmental stimuli (having a book at home and its number, and duration of book-reading, storytelling and singing) in children without any developmental delay in Tehran, was described, thus there was no information about ESR and its effect on children with developmental delay. Therefore, it is suggested that, in addition to conducting more extensive studies in the country, the quality and style of book reading for children should be addressed in the future in Iran.

Conclusion

According to the results of this study, ESR is considered as an early environmental stimulus and interventions in domains of communication and problem-solving in children. Therefore, the establishment of ESR culture among families and individuals in the Iranian community as a recreational and inseparable part of children's life from birth is recommended. As a result, due to the differences between culture and traditions among Iranian people, it is suggested more extensive studies be conducted on the quality, style of reading, and the views of parents and other caregivers on EI when dealing with reading books to children and its effects on ECD in Iran. The results of this study are only generalizable to Iranian children of Tehran.

Data availability

Dataset 1: ESR and ECD data from the East and West of Tehran. Variables are coded as follows: Birth rank: 1=The first child, 2=The second child and more; Caregivers: 1=mother, 2=father, 3=grandparents, 4=others; Age group: 0=3 to 17, 1=18 to 30; New Gestational age: 1<37 Week, 2≥37 Week; New mother educate: 1=diploma and below, 2=academic; New father educate: 1=diploma and below, 2=academic; New mother job: 1=Employed, 2=Housewife; New father job: 1=Employed, 2=Unemployed. DOI, 10.5256/f1000research.14078.d19865750

Formula

1n=(Z1α2)2×P(1P)d2

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 29 Mar 2018
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Sajedi F, Habibi E, Hatamizadeh N et al. Early storybook reading and childhood development: A cross-sectional study in Iran [version 1; peer review: 2 approved]. F1000Research 2018, 7:411 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.14078.1)
NOTE: If applicable, it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 29 Mar 2018
Views
8
Cite
Reviewer Report 13 Aug 2018
Leila Bazrafkan, Clinical Education Research Center, Shiraz University of Medical Sciences (SUMS), Shiraz, Iran 
Approved
VIEWS 8
The authors of the manuscript conducted a cross-sectional study to assess the state of early childhood development and early storybook reading in Iran. They reported a correlation between Early Storybook Reading (ESR) and early childhood development in communication development domain. ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Bazrafkan L. Reviewer Report For: Early storybook reading and childhood development: A cross-sectional study in Iran [version 1; peer review: 2 approved]. F1000Research 2018, 7:411 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.15314.r36960)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Views
11
Cite
Reviewer Report 17 Apr 2018
Marzieh Rostami Dovom, Reproductive Endocrinology Research Center (RERC), Research Institute for Endocrine Sciences, Shahid Beheshti University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran 
Approved
VIEWS 11
Thank you for sharing the manuscript entitled “Early storybook reading and childhood development: A cross-sectional study in Iran” with me. I read the paper carefully and find it is credible and appropriate for publication and it doesn’t have any major ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Rostami Dovom M. Reviewer Report For: Early storybook reading and childhood development: A cross-sectional study in Iran [version 1; peer review: 2 approved]. F1000Research 2018, 7:411 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.15314.r32641)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 19 Apr 2018
    elham habibi, Pediatric Neurorehabilitation Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran
    19 Apr 2018
    Author Response
    Dear Editor/Dear Referee
    Many thanks for your email and useful comments from the reviewer, which will certainly help to improve the paper.
    Referring to the comment #6, "In the data set, there ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 19 Apr 2018
    elham habibi, Pediatric Neurorehabilitation Research Center, University of Social Welfare and Rehabilitation Sciences, Tehran, Iran
    19 Apr 2018
    Author Response
    Dear Editor/Dear Referee
    Many thanks for your email and useful comments from the reviewer, which will certainly help to improve the paper.
    Referring to the comment #6, "In the data set, there ... Continue reading

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 29 Mar 2018
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.