Keywords
study within a trial, pen, financial incentive, recruitment, factorial, randomised controlled trial, older people, multimorbidity
This article is included in the Studies Within A Trial (SWAT) collection.
study within a trial, pen, financial incentive, recruitment, factorial, randomised controlled trial, older people, multimorbidity
Efficient recruitment to randomised controlled trials (RCTs) is important to achieve the target sample size and statistical power within the planned budget and time frame. Incentives, monetary or otherwise, are sometimes used to increase trial recruitment.1 We conducted a ‘study within a trial’ (SWAT) to evaluate the effects of including a small, unconditional financial incentive and/or a pen in the postal recruitment pack on the rate of randomisation into the host trial.
This 2x2 randomised factorial SWAT was embedded in the Gentle Years Yoga (GYY) trial, which is a multi-centre RCT of the clinical and cost-effectiveness of a yoga programme for older adults with multimorbidity (currently recruiting; ISRCTN13567538, registered 18/03/2019 https://www.isrctn.com/ISRCTN13567538). The SWAT was registered with the Northern Ireland Network for Trials Methodology Research SWAT repository on 01/04/2018 (SWAT94; https://www.qub.ac.uk/sites/TheNorthernIrelandNetworkforTrialsMethodologyResearch/SWATSWARInformation/Repositories/SWATStore/). The GYY trial, and its embedded sub-studies, has approval from the North East–York Research Ethics Committee, obtained on 24/04/2019 (19/NE/0072), and the Health Research Authority.
Patients who appeared to meet the GYY trial eligibility criteria and were to be mailed a recruitment pack, as identified by four participating GP practices, were eligible for inclusion in this methodological sub-study. The recruitment pack included an invitation letter, participant information sheet, consent form, screening form, and prepaid envelopes to return documentation to the York Trials Unit, University of York. A random sample of packs also included a £5 note and/or a pen (branded with the trial logo) as part of this SWAT. The packs were sent out in August 2019.
Financial incentives have been found to increase recruitment by 4% (95% CI -1–8%) in a meta-analysis.1 However, most of the included studies used an incentive of £100, which is larger than publicly funded trials can usually afford. There remains, therefore, uncertainty as to whether financial incentives should be used and, if so, what amount.
Offering a potential participant a gift such as a pen may make them more likely to take up the invitation to enrol in a trial. It is also possible that the convenience of having a pen to hand upon receipt of the invitation may help facilitate a swifter response. However, a previous SWAT conducted by the York Trials Unit evaluated the use of pens as an incentive for recruitment into the OTIS trial of older adults and showed no difference on randomisation rate (pen 4.5%; no pen 4.3%, odds ratio (OR) 1.04, 95% CI 0.65–1.67, p = 0.86), screening rate (pen 14.2%, no pen 11.7%, OR 1.25, 95% CI 0.94–1.67, p = 0.12), or time to return screening form (hazard ratio (HR) 1.23, 95% CI 0.94–1.60, p = 0.13).2 To our knowledge, this is the only previous RCT to evaluate pens to increase trial recruitment, so more evidence is needed; hence, we conducted this SWAT.
The primary outcome was randomisation into the host GYY trial. Secondary outcomes were return, and time to return, of a screening form to the York Trials Unit. The cost per additional participant recruited was calculated for each intervention.
Due to financial restrictions, we could afford to involve a sample of 850 recruitment packs in this SWAT. This would give 80% power (two-sided α=0.05) to detect a difference in recruitment rate of 4% (from 3% to 7%) for either of the interventions, relative to not receiving that intervention.
Block randomisation of size 4 was used to allocate recruitment packs 1:1:1:1 to: no pen or £5; £5 only; pen only; or pen and £5. A trial statistician, not involved in the production of recruitment packs or recruitment of participants, generated the sequence using Stata v15 (RRID: SCR_012763). Stata is a proprietary software but an open-access alternative in which the sequence could have been generated is Microsoft Excel (RRID: SCR_016137).
The statisticians were not blinded to allocation. Similarly, due to the nature of the interventions, participants could not be blinded to their allocation; however, they were not specifically informed about the SWAT nor that the incentive they received had been chosen through a process of randomisation. Specific consent for the trial was not required by the Research Ethics Committee, as it was considered low risk. Written informed consent for the main trial was obtained from all participants who agreed to take part in GYY.
The primary comparisons in this trial are the main effects of being sent a pen, and of being sent a £5 note. Returning a screening form and being randomised into the GYY trial were both analysed using multivariable logistic regression, including the two interventions (pen and £5). Time to return the screening form (in days from the date the recruitment pack was sent out to the date it was returned) was analysed using Cox proportional hazards regression. Screening forms that were not returned were censored at eight weeks after they were sent out. These analyses provide an estimate of the average effect of each intervention, assuming there is no interaction between them. In secondary analyses, the interaction between the two interventions was tested by extending the original models to include the interaction term. Analyses were conducted in Stata v16 (RRID: SCR_012763). An open-access alternative that can perform an equivalent function to Stata for analysis is R, a free software environment for statistical computing and graphics (RRID: SCR_001905).
In total, 852 allocations were generated but, due to one of the participating GP practices having a shorter mailing list than anticipated, only 818 (96.0%) were used (Table 1; Figure 1). In these analyses, the potential participants who were sent a pen (n = 409) consist of the group who received a pen and a £5 note (n = 203), and the group who received a pen only (n = 206). These are compared with those who were not sent a pen (n = 409), consisting of the group who were only sent a £5 note (n = 206), and the group who were sent neither a pen nor £5 (n = 203). Similarly, those who were sent £5 (n = 409) consist of the group sent both a pen and £5 (n = 203), and the group sent £5 only (n = 206). These are compared with the potential participants who were not sent £5 (n = 409), consisting of the group who were sent a pen only (n = 206), and the group who were sent neither a pen nor £5 (n = 203).
Pen | No pen | Total | |
---|---|---|---|
£5 | 203 | 206 | 409 |
No £5 | 206 | 203 | 409 |
Total | 409 | 409 | 818 |
26 (3.2%) SWAT participants were randomised into the host trial (Table 2). There was no evidence that randomisation rates were improved by including a pen (pen: 15/409, 3.7%; no pen: 11/409, 2.7%; OR 1.38, 95% CI 0.63–3.04, p = 0.43) or £5 (£5: 14/409, 3.4%; no £5: 12/409, 2.9%; OR 1.18, 95% CI 0.54–2.57, p = 0.69) in the recruitment packs. The interaction between the interventions was investigated as a secondary analysis and was not found to be statistically significant (interaction coefficient 0.98, 95% CI 0.20–4.79, p = 0.98). In a meta-analysis with the OTIS SWAT, the pooled OR associated with receipt of a pen was 1.12 (95% CI 0.75–1.67, p = 0.58) (Figure 2).
Pen (n = 409) | No pen (n = 409) | £5 (n = 409) | No £5 (n = 409) | Interaction coefficient (95% CI), p-valuec | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Returned screening form, n (%) | 66 (16.1) | 61 (14.9) | 77 (18.8) | 50 (12.2) | 1.66 (0.76–3.60), 0.20 |
Adjusted odds ratioa (95% CI), p-value | 1.10 (0.75–1.61), 0.61 | 1.67 (1.13–2.45), 0.01 | |||
Time to return (days)b, median (IQR) | 11 (9–14) | 11 (7–18) | 11 (10–18) | 8.5 (7–14) | 1.56 (0.76–3.19), 0.22 |
Adjusted hazards ratioa (95% CI), p-value | 1.09 (0.77–1.55), 0.61 | 1.56 (1.09–2.22), 0.02 | |||
Randomised, n (%) | 15 (3.7) | 11 (2.7) | 14 (3.4) | 12 (2.9) | 0.98 (0.20–4.79), 0.98 |
Adjusted odds ratioa (95% CI), p-value | 1.38 (0.63–3.04), 0.43 | 1.18 (0.54–2.57), 0.69 |
There was no evidence that including a pen increased the likelihood of returning a screening form (pen: 66/409, 16.1%; no pen: 61/409, 14.9%; OR 1.10; 95% CI 0.75–1.61, p = 0.61), but there was strong evidence for including £5 (£5: 77/409, 18.8%; no £5: 50/409, 12.2%; OR 1.67; 95% CI 1.13–2.45, p = 0.01). The interaction between the interventions was investigated as a secondary analysis and was not found to be statistically significant (interaction coefficient 1.66, 95% CI 0.76–3.60, p = 0.20).
There was no evidence of a difference in time to return a screening form associated with inclusion of a pen (HR 1.09; 95% CI 0.77–1.55, p = 0.61), but including £5 decreased the time to return a screening form (HR 1.56; 95% CI 1.09–2.22, p=0.02). See Kaplan–Meier plots (Figure 3). The Grambsch and Therneau test did not indicate deviation from the proportional hazards assumption.3 The interaction between the interventions was investigated as a secondary analysis and was not found to be statistically significant (interaction coefficient 1.56, 95% CI 0.76–3.19, p = 0.22).
The additional cost of including a pen in the postal mailout was £0.32; the inclusion of £5 additionally cost only the value of the note itself. Given the 1% increase in participants randomised when sent a pen, 100 (1/0.01) potential participants would need to be sent a pen to recruit one additional participant at a cost of £32 (100×£0.32). We would need to send 200 participants £5, at a cost of £1000, to recruit one extra participant.
Participants sent a pen or £5 were marginally more likely to be randomised into the GYY trial than those who did not receive the incentive; however, the differences were not statistically significant. The randomisation rates in the ‘no intervention’ arms (2.7% and 2.9%) were similar to the 3% assumed in the sample size calculation but the observed group differences were smaller than the 4% difference we were powered for; therefore, this SWAT was underpowered to detect the differences observed.
There was little or no evidence that sending a pen increased the likelihood of returning a screening form or decreasing time to return the form.
Although no statistically significant interactions between the pen and £5 were observed, this cannot be ruled out as the sample size of this trial was likely insufficient to be powered to detect an interaction.
A small, monetary incentive was effective at prompting return of the screening form, and of a swifter return, but it did not result in more participants being randomised into the host trial. Some anecdotal evidence from the GYY trial’s process evaluation suggested participants felt it unnecessary to receive £5 with their recruitment pack as they would willingly have joined the trial without this purely to help themselves, others and the research. In addition, this may have caused potential confusion if participants discussed receiving £5 during their yoga sessions as to why some received it and some did not. Such sentiments were not expressed in relation to being sent a pen, potentially suggesting that people view non-monetary incentives differently (more like a gift) than monetary incentives. Since it is relatively costly, we do not recommend this intervention for use to increase recruitment in older adults with multimorbidity.
Pens are cheaper but provided little evidence of benefit. If the observed effect of a 1% difference was true then we would need sufficient SWATs to provide an overall sample size of around 11,000 participants to confirm this. Because the extra cost of recruiting an additional participant is relatively small, more SWATs are required to assess whether this difference is a true effect, since sending pens could be a cost-effective intervention for recruitment.
OSF: Underlying data for ‘A 2x2 randomised factorial SWAT of the use of a pen and small, financial incentive to improve recruitment rates in a randomised controlled trial of yoga for older adults with multimorbidity’. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/2CJZH.4
This project contains the following underlying data:
Data file 1. GYY recruitment SWAT csv data.csv
Data file 2. GYY recruitment SWAT Stata data.dta
Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Zero “No rights reserved” data waiver (CC0 1.0 Public domain dedication).
OSF: CONSORT checklist for ‘A 2x2 randomised factorial SWAT of the use of a pen and small, financial incentive to improve recruitment rates in a randomised controlled trial of yoga for older adults with multimorbidity’. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/EU68F.5
Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Zero “No rights reserved” data waiver (CC0 1.0 Public domain dedication).
We would like to acknowledge the contributions of Professor Tim Rapley (Professor of Applied Health Care Research, Department of Social Work, Education and Community Wellbeing, Northumbria University) and Helen Tilbrook (York Trials Unit, University of York), who were co-applicants on the initial GYY trial grant proposal.
Views | Downloads | |
---|---|---|
F1000Research | - | - |
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
|
- | - |
Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?
Yes
Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?
Yes
Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?
No
If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?
Partly
Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?
Yes
Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?
Partly
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: Trial Methodology; Epidemiology
Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?
Yes
Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?
Yes
Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?
Partly
If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?
Yes
Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?
Partly
Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?
Yes
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: Health psychology
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Invited Reviewers | ||
---|---|---|
1 | 2 | |
Version 2 (revision) 04 Mar 22 |
read | |
Version 1 27 Apr 21 |
read | read |
Provide sufficient details of any financial or non-financial competing interests to enable users to assess whether your comments might lead a reasonable person to question your impartiality. Consider the following examples, but note that this is not an exhaustive list:
Sign up for content alerts and receive a weekly or monthly email with all newly published articles
Already registered? Sign in
The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.
You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.
You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.
If your email address is registered with us, we will email you instructions to reset your password.
If you think you should have received this email but it has not arrived, please check your spam filters and/or contact for further assistance.
Comments on this article Comments (0)