Keywords
Teaching strategies, Critical thinking, Health, Systematic reviews
We conducted an overview of systematic reviews about the effects of teaching strategies that can be used to teach primary and secondary school students to think critically. Our objective was to inform decisions about what teaching strategies to use in resources that we developed to teach critical thinking about health in secondary schools.
We mapped characteristics of systematic reviews of teaching strategies and summarised findings from the most relevant reviews to teaching students to think critically about health. We included reviews that assessed the effects of teaching strategies that could potentially be used in primary or secondary schools to teach students to think critically, had a Methods section with explicit selection criteria, reported at least one outcome measure of the ability to undertake one of four basic types of cognitive tasks (memory, procedural, comprehension, or opinion), and were published after 1999.
We included 326 systematic reviews. The reviews evaluated a wide range of teaching strategies for a variety of purposes. Important limitations of the reviews included not considering adverse effects (99% of the reviews), not assessing the risk of bias for included studies (93% of the reviews), and not assessing the credibility of subgroup effects (100% of the reviews). We summarised the findings for 37 teaching strategies that we considered most relevant. The certainty of the evidence of the effects varied from very low to moderate. We used 12 of the strategies in resources that we developed to teach secondary students to think critically about health.
A tremendous amount of work has gone into evaluating the effects of a wide range of teaching strategies. The results of this research can inform decisions about how to teach critical thinking and future research. However, well-designed, up-to-date systematic reviews are still needed for many teaching strategies.
Teaching strategies, Critical thinking, Health, Systematic reviews
We added information to the introduction to better situate this study within the broader research context. We also added a limitation that we did not consider publication bias in the included reviews.
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Keji Fan
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Sofia Avgitidou
We designed the Be Smart about your Health resources for secondary school teachers and their students (in the 8th or 9th year of school, age 13-15) in Kenya, Rwanda, and Uganda.1 The aim of these resources is to help students cope with the abundance of information and misinformation about how to care for their health. Critical thinking is important for making informed choices and it is a core competency in the curricula for all three countries.2–4 However, it is often not taught in the classroom, partly due to a lack of carefully developed educational resources, and a lack of information about effective teaching strategies. The Be Smart about your Health resources were developed to help fill this gap. These resources focus on critical thinking about health actions – things that people do to care for their health or the health of others. Critical thinking about health actions is especially important because:
• Good health is essential for daily life and depends on informed health choices, which depend on critical thinking about health actions.
• Health is important to everyone, which makes it a good starting point for learning critical thinking skills that students can transfer to other subjects, such as the environment.5
The Be Smart about your Health resources focus on nine key concepts that people need to understand and apply when deciding what to believe about the effects of health actions and what to do.6,7 We undertook this overview of systematic reviews of the effects of teaching strategies to inform the design those specific resources. However, the results from this review can also be applied more broadly to inform the development of other critical thinking learning resources. Given the growing use of large language model and artificial intelligence that young people are exposed to, coupled with a deluge of mis/disinformation, critical thinking skills are increasingly important to ensure that young people have the necessary skills to identify reliable evidence and make informed choices. In response, many educational systems have begun to recognize the importance of critical thinking by including these skills in curriculum reforms, national and international standardized tests (e.g., Programme for International Student Assessment; PISA), and by incorporating problem-based learning approaches in the classroom.8 However, the implementation of teaching strategies to facilitate and support the development of critical thinking skills varies widely across subjects, schools and countries.9,10
Learning to think critically is widely held to be an aim of education.11 However, there is not agreement on the definition of “critical thinking”, or which frameworks (conceptual structures intended to serve as a support or guide) best support critical thinking.12–16 Ennis has defined critical thinking as “reasoned, reflective thinking focused on deciding what to believe or do”,17 and we use that definition in this overview.18
Thinking evolved to help us choose what to do to achieve our goals after taking account of estimates of the likely effects of our actions.19 A fundamental goal of critical thinking is to improve decision making by increasing the likelihood that we will believe and act on those claims that are more likely to help us achieve our goals.20
Definitions of teaching (instructional, or pedagogical) strategies (techniques, methods, or approaches) vary. Some authors distinguish between strategies, techniques, methods, and approaches. However, there is overlap in how these terms are used. Our focus is on “different ways of helping students to learn - that is, different ways of helping them to achieve the learning outcomes that [teachers] have decided are important”.21 We refer to these as “teaching strategies”.
There are several lists of teaching strategies, organised in different ways.21–27 Beck surveyed 25 teacher education textbooks and was unable to find two similar lists of teaching strategies.22 Pomerance and colleagues reviewed 48 textbooks for elementary and secondary teacher training and found that none of the textbooks accurately described six fundamental instructional strategies.27 At most, only two of the six were covered in any textbook, and when textbooks did mention the strategies (allowing for a broad range of terminology and descriptions), the discussion could be as brief as 1-2 sentences.
Although our specific interest is in primary and secondary school students and critical thinking outcomes, we have not limited this overview to that population or those outcome measures. There are four reasons for this. First, there are not many reviews that focus specifically on critical thinking.28–39 To the extent that those reviews do consider the effects of specific strategies, they tend to be broad categories and comparisons of strategies tend to be made indirectly (in meta-regression analyses). For example, Abrami and colleagues explored differences in the effect of three types of instruction (dialogue; authentic or anchored instruction; and mentoring, coaching, or tutoring) across 341 comparisons with different populations, outcome measures, and study designs.28 Thus, an overview that only included critical thinking as an outcome would be limited.
Second, although some learning outcomes may be of little relevance to learning to think critically, it is difficult to specify a priori which outcomes are completely irrelevant and which might provide useful information despite not directly measuring critical thinking. For example, on the one hand it can be argued that outcome measures that only require retention of knowledge are irrelevant to critical thinking. On the other hand, it is important that students have knowledge of key concepts (principles for critical thinking)7,40 and that they retain that knowledge. Other outcome measures, such as reading comprehension or understanding of science texts, are dependent on a range of factors in addition to critical thinking.
Third, many reviews are not limited to primary or secondary school interventions and may or may not explore differences in effects across different students. Although some teaching strategies might be expected to have different effects for different types of students, it is uncertain whether this is the case. Starting out with an overly narrow focus in terms of the students could result in an overview that is far less informative than it might otherwise be.
Fourth, we were unsure how many potentially useful systematic reviews of teaching strategies there were and what the characteristics of those reviews were.
For these reasons, we conducted an overview to characterise the range of systematic reviews of teaching strategies that can potentially inform the design of resources to help primary and secondary school students learn to think critically. The overview may be useful to researchers, teachers, policymakers, and others with an interest in other learning outcomes and students. It enabled us to make an informed decision about which reviews were most useful for our specific interests.1
Our primary objectives were to provide an overview of what is known from systematic reviews about the effects of teaching strategies that can be used to teach primary and secondary school students to think critically and inform the design of educational resources (the Be Smart about your Health resources) to teach lower secondary school students in East Africa to think critically about health claims and choices.
Secondary objectives were to:
• Map characteristics of systematic reviews of teaching strategies
• Identify needs and priorities to assess teaching strategies based on the findings of the included systematic reviews
• Identify needs and priorities to assess systematic reviews of the effects of teaching strategies
• Inform the development of a framework for types of teaching strategies
The protocol for this overview was published in Zenodo in December 2019.41
We included systematic reviews that:
• assess the effects of teaching strategies (different ways of helping students to learn) that can potentially be used in primary or secondary schools to teach students to think critically,
• have a Methods section with explicit selection criteria,
• report at least one outcome measure of the ability to undertake one of four basic types of cognitive tasks (memory, procedural, comprehension, or opinion),42 and
• were published after 1999.
We excluded reviews of teaching strategies that were restricted to:
• professional students (e.g., medical or nursing students) other than teacher training
• special education (teaching children and youth with disabilities)
• creative or physical skills such as artistic, cooking, musical, or physical skills
Doyle defined the four basic types of cognitive tasks noted above as follows42:
1. memory tasks in which students are expected to recognize or reproduce information previously encountered (e.g., memorize a list of spelling words or lines from a poem);
2. procedural or routine tasks in which students are expected to apply a standardized and predictable formula or algorithm to generate answers (e.g., solve a set of subtraction problems);
3. comprehension or understanding tasks in which students are expected to (a) recognize transformed or paraphrased versions of information previously encountered, (b) apply procedures to new problems or decide from among several procedures those which are applicable to a particular problem (e.g., solve “word problems” in mathematics), or (c) draw inferences from previously encountered information or procedures (e.g., make predictions about a chemical reaction or devise an alternative formula for squaring a number);
4. opinion tasks in which students are expected to state a preference for something (e.g., select a favourite short story).
These tasks roughly correspond with Bloom’s taxonomy, which has six main categories of intellectual abilities and skills: knowledge, comprehension, application, analysis, synthesis, and evaluation.43 Bloom’s taxonomy is well known and has clear definitions, but it difficult to make clear distinctions between the higher-order categories.14 For the purposes of this overview, we considered any task that requires judgement (‘evaluation’ in Bloom’s taxonomy) as ‘opinion tasks’ including judgements about what to believe and what to do.
We created an initial list of potentially relevant teaching strategies (Box 1) by reviewing several lists.21–26,44 We started with Beck’s taxonomy,22 which we adapted and reorganised, considering other teaching strategies and ways of categorising these. We continued to develop this list of terms iteratively, based on the literature that we retrieved and input from educational researchers and teachers.
Didactic strategies (instruction in which information is presented directly from the teacher to the student, in which the teacher selects the topic, controls instructional stimuli, obligates a response from the student, evaluates responses, and provides reinforcement for correct responses and feedback for incorrect ones)
Direct instruction, lectures, textbooks, picture books, audio-visual aids, podcasts, multimedia instruction, demonstration, modelling, mini lessons, reading, graphic presentations, combined graphic and verbal presentations, narrative text, comics, humour, scaffolding, pre-teaching vocabulary, link abstract concepts with concrete representations
Questioning techniques (methods used for constructing and presenting questions in order to promote effective discussions and learning or to elicit information)
Socratic method, open ended questions, closed questions, interviewing, prompting, probing, redirecting, wait time, clickers, pose probing questions, oral or written reports, cloze, “assess to boost retention”, quizzes, ask and answer deep questions
Discussion strategies
Classroom discussion, small group discussion, buzz sessions, brainstorming, round table, debate, structured controversy, magic circle, fishbowl dialogue, four sides/corners strategy, reflective discussion, flipped classroom
Role playing
Read aloud, readers’ theatre, dramatic play, storytelling, mock trial, simulation, learning games, public speaking, and speech writing
Problem-based learning
Enquiry-based learning, exploration-based learning, student research, research projects, learning through experimentation, science fairs, science Olympics, using case studies to teach, laboratory teaching methods, field trips, discovery learning, analytic memo, concept attainment, concept formation, concept maps, graphic organizer, knowledge map, cognitive organiser, mind mapping, structured overview, “repeatedly alternating problems with their solutions provided and problems that students must solve”
Repetition and progression
Distributed practice, space learning over time, spaced learning, pacing, learning targets, learning progression, competency-based learning, sequential approach, explicit teaching, interdisciplinary teaching
Assessment techniques
Feedback, classroom assessment techniques, formative assessment, background knowledge probe, the one-minute paper, traffic light cards, muddiest point, what’s the principle, problem recognition task, student generated test questions, classroom opinion polls, directed paraphrasing, pro and con grid, student goals ranking, course-related interest and skills checklist, self-diagnostic learning logs, misconception/preconception check, empty outlines, invented dialogues, diagnostic teaching, precision teaching
Collaborative learning (a situation in which two or more people learn or attempt to learn something together)
Dyads, partners, cross/multi-age groups, ability and interest groups, heterogeneous groups, homogeneous groups, cooperative learning, heads together strategy, numbered heads together strategy, jigsaw teaching technique, team learning, peer teaching, peer partner learning, reciprocal teaching, readers’ workshop, reading buddies, think-ink-pair-share learning strategy, think-pair-share learning strategy, heterogeneous grouping, homogeneous grouping, multiple intelligences activities
Individual learning
Individualized instruction, learner-controlled instruction, self-paced learning, independent study, programmed learning, contract learning, mastery learning, tutorial instruction, learning centres, menus, course packets, teaching tailored to students’ learning styles, Dalton plan, writing, writing to inform, paraphrasing, pause and reflect, journal writing, homework, practice, anchor activities
E-learning (using electronic devices, applications, or processes to acquire or transfer knowledge, attitudes, or skills through study, instruction, or experience)
Online learning, web-based learning, web-based instruction, Web Quest, computer-based training, mobile learning, virtual classrooms, webinars, interactive e-lessons, online discussions, electronic simulations, audio/video recording
In-service teacher training
Microteaching, powerful pedagogical strategies, team teaching, scaffolding, peer teaching, teachers’ guides, or any of the other teaching strategies listed above
Starting with the terms in Box 1, we developed search strategies for Education Research Complete (EBSCO) and for Education Resources Information Center (ERIC) (Ovid).73 The searches were conducted October 13, 2019 (EBSCO) and October 29, 2019 (ERIC). We updated the searches March 15, 2022 (ERIC) and March 22, 2022 (EBSCO).
Two authors independently screened the titles and abstracts identified in October 2019 to identify systematic reviews that met our inclusion criteria. Disagreements were resolved by discussion, involving a third author if needed. We pilot tested the selection criteria on a sample of 100 records as training, before screening the search results. We retrieved the full text of articles that appeared to meet the selection criteria and two authors independently assessed each article for inclusion. Cochrane Response45 screened the updated search and selected reviews for inclusion in the same way.
For each systematic review included in the overview two review authors independently collected the following data:
• Year of the last search
• Number of included studies
• Countries where included studies were done
• School subjects
• Education level of participants
• Age of participants
• Teaching strategies that were evaluated and their definitions
• Outcome measures included in the review
• Consideration of adverse effects
• Included study designs (randomized trials, non-randomized studies, mixed)
• Assessment of the risk of bias for effect estimates
• Limitations of estimates for effect modifiers considered by the review authors
Cochrane Response collected the data for included reviews identified by the updated search in the same way.
We mapped characteristics of the included systematic reviews. We used a framework thematic synthesis approach to categorise the teaching strategies.46,47 This entailed four stages: familiarisation, coding, charting and interpretation of the data. We started with the framework shown in Box 1. The definitions and boundaries of each category of strategies were discussed among the review authors, and the framework was revised in line with categories that emerged from the data.
We assessed the relevance of each included review for teaching critical thinking in primary and secondary schools. These judgements were discussed by the review team and a consensus was reached on the systematic reviews that were most relevant to the design of the Be Smart about your Health resources. For each teaching strategy that we considered relevant, one review author prepared a summary based on the included systematic reviews. The other authors reviewed and edited those summaries. Each summary included an explanation of the strategy, when and why it should be used, a bottom line, and a judgement of the certainty of the evidence for the bottom-line using Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation (GRADE) criteria.48,49 We did not do this for the reviews identified by the updated search, which was conducted after the Be Smart about your Health resources had been designed and pilot tested.
We identified 9280 records - 6499 records in the search conducted in October 2019 and 2781 records in the updated search conducted in March 2022 ( Figure 1). After removing duplicates, we screened a total of 8110 records and assessed 685 full-text reports for eligibility. We included a total of 326 systematic reviews of teaching strategies that reported at least one quantitative cognitive outcome measure applicable to primary or secondary schools.73
The number of systematic reviews of teaching strategies published each year increased from two in 2000 to 56 in 2020 ( Figure 2). Just over 40% of the reviews (132 reviews; 40.5%) were published in seven journals: Review of Educational Research (40; 12.3%), Educational Research Review (25; 7.7%), Educational Psychology Review (17; 5.2%), Computers & Education (15; 4.6%), Journal of Educational Psychology (15; 4.6%), Journal of Computer Assisted Learning (10; 3.1%), and Journal of Educational Computing Research (10; 3.1%).
Among the 226 reviews (69.3%) that reported the year of the last search, the difference between the year of publication and the year of the last search was two or more years for 151 reviews (64.5%). The year of the last search was not reported for 100 (30.7%) of the included reviews.
The reviews included between 5 and 1105 studies (median 34). The studies were conducted in over 60 different countries. Most studies were conducted in high-income countries. Studies from 16 countries were included in 10 or more reviews ( Table 1). Studies from the USA were included in at least 110 reviews (32.8%). However, 204 reviews (62.6%) did not report the countries in which included studies were conducted and other reviews only partially reported the countries in which included studies were conducted.
The education level of the included participants varied widely within and across reviews ( Table 2). Over 100 of the included reviews (104; 31.9%) included studies with participants ranging from preschool or primary school through university. Only 11.7% (38) of the reviews focused specifically on primary, middle, or secondary school. The reviews that included multiple education levels frequently did not explore education level as an effect modifier. Thirty-nine reviews (12.0%) did not report the education level of participants in the included studies or use educational level as a criterion for including studies in the review. Most of the included reviews (262; 80.4%) did not report the ages of participants in included studies or the ages corresponding to the included education levels.
Many of the reviews (112; 34.4%) included studies in multiple school subjects, often without exploring the subject as a potential effect modifier ( Table 3). Nearly one third (100; 30.7%) of the reviews focused on math and science topics. Many of the reviews (92; 28.2%) did not report the subjects that were the focus of the included studies.
School subjects | Systematic reviews | |
---|---|---|
n | % | |
Multiple subjects | 113 | 34.7% |
Multiple science, math & science, or STEM subjects* | 57 | 17.5% |
Mathematics subjects | 38 | 11.7% |
Single science subjects | 5 | 1.5% |
Language or literacy | 21 | 6.4% |
Not reported | 92 | 28.2% |
Total | 326 | 100% |
The included reviews addressed a wide range of teaching strategies, often using different terms to describe similar strategies, and using different definitions.73 We grouped the teaching strategies into 11 broad categories ( Table 4). Altogether, 110 reviews (33.7%) addressed the use of information and communication technology in education (“E-learning”). Half (55) of those reviews fit in a different category than E-learning category. For example, we included digital games in the category “games and role play”.
Twenty-two reviews addressed the use of games or gamification, most of which focused on digital games, and we found 16 reviews of flipped classrooms ( Table 5). On the other hand, we found only four reviews of teacher training that reported student outcomes, and only eight reviews of the effects of different approaches to questioning and prompting students.
The included reviews reported a wide range of measures of academic achievement (measured using standardized tests or grades) or learning (measured using researcher- or teacher-created tests). Twenty-six reviews (8.0%) reported a measure of transfer. Forty-six reviews (14.1%) reported a measure of higher order thinking, including critical thinking (11 reviews; 3.4%), problem solving (16; 4.9%), metacognition (8; 2.5%), and argumentation (5; 1.5%).
Three reviews mentioned adverse effects. One noted parenthetically in the authors’ conclusions that “We found … (and no significant adverse effects).” It did not refer to adverse effects anywhere else in the review. The other two reviews mentioned potential adverse effects (of parental involvement with homework, and of home environments). Two reviews mentioned practical disadvantages (student unfamiliarity and instructor start-up cost; and taking time away from instruction and students’ responses). Two reviews considered costs. Thirty-three reviews (10.1%) considered negative effects on the learning outcomes that were reported. The rest of the reviews (286; 87.7%) did not consider adverse effects, disadvantages, or costs.
The study designs included in the reviews often were not clear ( Table 6). Reviews frequently included “Experimental” and “quasi-experimental” studies without providing clear definitions. In some reviews, “experimental” clearly referred to studies using random allocation, but that was not always clear. “Quasi-experimental” could refer to a variety of different study designs. Similarly, when “quantitative” or “comparative” studies were included, the designs of the included studies often were not described.
Study designs | n | % |
---|---|---|
“Experimental" and "quasi-experimental”* | 157 | 48.2% |
“Experimental”* | 59 | 18.1% |
Quantitative (comparative)† | 31 | 9.5% |
“Quasi-experimental”* | 18 | 5.5% |
Quantitative and qualitative† | 21 | 6.4% |
Case studies | 4 | 1.2% |
Not clearly reported | 36 | 11.0% |
Total | 326 | 100% |
Twenty-four (7.4%) of the reviews assessed the risk of bias for included studies using explicit criteria. Twenty (6.1%) assessed “study quality” using explicit criteria, some of which are not directly related to the risk of bias, and four reviews (1.2%) did not clearly report the criteria that were used. Fifty-three reviews (20.2%) did not assess the risk of bias but included random allocation (53) or study design (13) as a potential effect moderator. The other 212 (65%) of the reviews did not address the risk of bias.
Most reviews (235; 72.1%) assessed potential effect moderators using univariate meta-regressions (173 reviews; 53.1%), multivariate meta-regression (25; 7.7%), or network meta-analysis (1 review). Thirty-six reviews (11.0%) reported effects for subgroups of included studies without a test of interaction (statistical significance of differences between subgroups). Of the 235 reviews that assessed potential effect modifiers, 39 (16.2%) addressed limitations of those analyses due to, for example, a lack of data (17 reviews), general limitations of moderator analyses (10 reviews), or potential confounding (7 reviews). None of the included reviews systematically assessed the credibility of reported subgroup effects.
We prepared plain language summaries for 37 teaching strategies that we judged to be most relevant to teaching critical thinking in primary and secondary schools, and one for teacher training.73 These summaries informed the choice of teaching strategies that we used in Be Smart about your Health. We included teaching strategies that could potentially be used to teach critical thinking, even though we did not find a systematic review that directly considered effects on critical thinking. We also included strategies that are relevant for learning new vocabulary (such as important new terms that are introduced in the resources) and for explaining and helping students to understand the key concepts that are taught in the resources.
For each summary we included when the review authors last searched for evidence. We also included a judgement about the certainty of the evidence, based on limitations of the included studies (our assessment based on information provided in the reviews), imprecision of the effect estimates, and inconsistency of results. We judged the certainty of the evidence to be moderate for 16 teaching strategies, low to moderate for one, low for 11, and very low for 10 ( Table 7). We did not judge the certainty of the evidence to by high for any of the teaching strategies.
We categorised the size of the effects from little or no effect to a large effect, as shown in Table 7. The 122 average effect sizes that we included in the summaries ranged from 0.01 to 1.68, median 0.42 ( Figure 3). We wrote short descriptions and “bottom-line statements” for the teaching strategies that we considered most relevant. The bottom-line statements are shown in Table 8. The bottom-line statements communicate the certainty of the evidence and the size of the effects (when these were available). We constructed them by adapting strategies for writing informative sentences to communicate evidence from systematic reviews.50
* References can be found in Oxman (2024).73
Although many of the reviews had important limitations, they provided a valuable starting point for selecting and implementing teaching strategies that we incorporated in our educational resources and that teachers potentially can use to teach critical thinking. Some teaching strategies require more training and experience of teachers than others. We included information about the need for teacher training or experience, whenever this was addressed in a review.
Hundreds of systematic reviews of teaching strategies have been produced over the past 20 years. They provide a valuable basis for informing decisions about the use of these strategies. However, these reviews have some important limitations, including:
• Inconsistent terminology and definitions
• Unnecessary duplication of efforts
• Failure to consider adverse effects
• Inadequate assessments of the risk of bias
• Inadequate assessments of the credibility of subgroup effects (effect moderators)
We developed and revised a framework for categorising teaching strategies that met our needs. It is uncertain whether that framework will prove useful to others. Some of the reviews included in our overview could be classified in more than one way. Nonetheless, we found the framework helpful for organising the reviews and the teaching strategies and making sense of what is known. It would be helpful for users of this research if researchers studying the effects of teaching strategies could reach a consensus on a taxonomy. Similarly, it would be helpful if agreement could be reached on the terminology that is used to characterize outcome measures. Coordination by an organisation such as the Campbell Collaboration might help to reduce unnecessary duplication of efforts and make it easier to update the reviews. It might also help reduce the risk of bias in systematic reviews of teaching strategies.51
Only three of the systematic reviews included in this overview even mentioned adverse effects. Researchers commonly fail to consider potential adverse effects of educational and public health interventions.52–55 An adverse effect on an intended outcome is sometimes found, but other types of adverse effects are rarely considered, including misunderstanding, misapplication of learning, distrust, cognitive dissonance, stress, inequities, conflict, and wasted time or resources.
Many of the systematic reviews did not assess the risk of bias for studies included in the reviews using explicit criteria. This made it difficult to judge the certainty of the evidence, i.e., confidence in the reported effect estimates.56–58 In addition, none of the reviews systematically assessed the credibility of reported subgroup (moderator) effects.59–61 The credibility of most subgroup claims in randomized trials, even strong claims, is usually low.62,63
Although we have included effect sizes in our summaries of strategies that we considered most relevant to teaching critical thinking in primary and secondary schools, the average effect sizes summarised in Figure 3, as well as those reported in other overviews such as Hattie’s,64 are difficult to interpret for several reasons in addition to those noted above, including:
For these reasons, the reported effect sizes should not be used to compare the effectiveness of different teaching strategies and firm conclusions cannot be drawn about the magnitude of effects for most of the teaching strategies.
Nonetheless, the findings of the systematic reviews that we considered most relevant for teaching critical thinking in primary and secondary schools helped to inform the design of the Be Smart about your Health resources. Because we developed the resources in East Africa, many of the teaching strategies that we considered potentially relevant in other settings were not practical in low-resource settings, particularly ones that depend on the availability of digital technology. We initially pilot tested resources that used different teaching strategies for each lesson. However, this was inefficient, since the strategies were new to both teachers and students, and this distracted from the learning goals of the lessons. Therefore, we selected a small number of strategies that we used consistently across lessons and only varied the main lesson strategy a few times ( Table 9).1
Strategies used across lessons | Strategies used in individual lessons |
---|---|
|
|
The Be Smart about your Health intervention is being evaluated in randomized trials and process evaluations in Kenya, Rwanda, and Uganda.67–72
Some of the teaching strategies included in this review may be more effective for some subjects than others, however this is outside the scope of this review, but could benefit from further investigation.
Limitations of this overview and our findings include:
• The searches were limited to two databases (Education Research Complete (EBSCO) and Education Resources Information Center (ERIC).
• Coding the included reviews was challenging and there were frequent disagreements.
• We did not systematically assess the risk of bias for included reviews.
• The assessments of the certainty of the evidence for reported effects were based on information provided in the included reviews, which was frequently incomplete.
• We did not assess the relevance of the reviews identified by the updated search or summarise the findings for any of those reviews.
• We did not consider publication bias in the included reviews. It is uncertain whether publication bias affected the results of the included reviews. To the extent it did, the results may overestimate the effects of the interventions.
A coordinated effort by educational researchers and education professionals is needed to:
• Reach a consensus on terminology and definitions of teaching strategies,
• Set priorities for systematic reviews of teaching strategies for which a high quality, up-to-date systematic review cannot be found, and
• Reduce unnecessary duplication of efforts.
Investigators preparing prioritised systematic reviews should consider the limitations that we identified in the included reviews when preparing a protocol. They should consider potential adverse effects, systematically assess the risk of bias for included studies, and systematically assess the credibility of subgroup effects (effect modifiers).
Teachers and developers of educational resources for teaching critical thinking in primary and secondary schools may want to consider using some of the teaching strategies that we have judged to be most relevant. There are important uncertainties about the effects of most of those teaching strategies, and there may be other teaching strategies worth considering for which we did not find a systematic review. Nonetheless, the teaching strategies summarised provide a useful starting point for identifying and selecting potentially useful teaching strategies.73
A tremendous amount of work has gone into evaluating the effects of a wide range of teaching strategies. The results of this research can inform decisions about how to teach critical thinking or achieve other learning goals, as well as decisions about further research to evaluate the effects of teaching strategies. However, well-designed, up-to-date systematic reviews are still needed for many teaching strategies. Coordination of efforts could help to address this need.
Zenodo: Dataset for “The effects of teaching strategies on learning to think critically in primary and secondary schools: an overview of systematic reviews”, https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.14001300.73
This project contains the following underlying data:
Zenodo: Dataset for “The effects of teaching strategies on learning to think critically in primary and secondary schools: an overview of systematic reviews”, https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.14001300.73
This project contains the following extended data:
- Education Research Complete search strategy.docx
- ERIC search strategy.docx
- PRISMA_2020_checklist teaching strategies.docx
- Strategies for teaching critical thinking.docx
Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (CC-BY 4.0).
We would like to thank Astrid Dahlgren and Nelson Sewankambo for their contributions to the protocol; Marit Johansen for developing the search strategies; Iain Chalmers, Steven Higgins, Monica Melby-Lervåg, and Matt Oxman for providing feedback on the protocol; and Monica Melby-Lervåg and Jonathan Sharples for providing feedback on an earlier version of this manuscript.
Views | Downloads | |
---|---|---|
F1000Research | - | - |
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
|
- | - |
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Are the rationale for, and objectives of, the Systematic Review clearly stated?
Yes
Are sufficient details of the methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?
Yes
Is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?
Yes
Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results presented in the review?
Yes
If this is a Living Systematic Review, is the ‘living’ method appropriate and is the search schedule clearly defined and justified? (‘Living Systematic Review’ or a variation of this term should be included in the title.)
Yes
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: Systematic reviews; evidence synthesis; critical thinking instruction; randomised controlled trials; evidence-based education
Are the rationale for, and objectives of, the Systematic Review clearly stated?
Yes
Are sufficient details of the methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?
Yes
Is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?
Yes
Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results presented in the review?
Yes
If this is a Living Systematic Review, is the ‘living’ method appropriate and is the search schedule clearly defined and justified? (‘Living Systematic Review’ or a variation of this term should be included in the title.)
Not applicable
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: Initial and in-service teacher education, professional learning, transformation of education and educational change, democratic and participatory education
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Invited Reviewers | ||
---|---|---|
1 | 2 | |
Version 2 (revision) 16 Apr 25 |
read | |
Version 1 26 Nov 24 |
read | read |
Provide sufficient details of any financial or non-financial competing interests to enable users to assess whether your comments might lead a reasonable person to question your impartiality. Consider the following examples, but note that this is not an exhaustive list:
Sign up for content alerts and receive a weekly or monthly email with all newly published articles
Already registered? Sign in
The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.
You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.
You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.
If your email address is registered with us, we will email you instructions to reset your password.
If you think you should have received this email but it has not arrived, please check your spam filters and/or contact for further assistance.
Comments on this article Comments (0)