ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Research Article

Shining Light into Adolescent HIV Neuroplasticity: A Study of the Prefrontal Cortex Using Functional Near Infrared Spectrometry

[version 1; peer review: awaiting peer review]
PUBLISHED 29 Sep 2025
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS AWAITING PEER REVIEW

Abstract

Background

The human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) crosses the blood-brain barrier, compromising cortical networks and cognition. Neuropharmacological intervention in the form of combination antiretroviral therapy (cART) does not reverse cognitive decline in pediatric and adolescent HIV. In search of options to enhance cognition in affected adolescents, we evaluated the effectiveness of attention training.

Methods

The training was evaluated by pairing participants’ outcomes on the Stroop Colour Word Test (SCWT), a test of attention and inhibitory control, to brain hemodynamic responses measured by functional near-infrared spectrometry (fNIRS) both prior to, and after, the training. Changes in oxygenated haemoglobin in the prefrontal cortex during the SCWT were compared in 15 HIV+ participants who received attention training with that of 13 HIV+ participants who did not receive the intervention.

Results

Intragroup analyses showed differences in oxygenated haemoglobin levels between congruent and incongruent conditions of the SCWT only for participants who received the attention training. Specifically, there was a significant decrease in oxygenated haemoglobin in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and frontopolar brain areas in the training group.

Conclusions

Findings suggest that customised attention training maybe an effective complementary adjunct to cART for children and adolescents living with HIV.

Keywords

adolescent HIV, attention, cART, fNIRS, prefrontal cortex

Introduction

The human immunodeficiency virus (HIV) is a significant global pandemic, with approximately 39 million people living with the virus. South Africa has the highest burden of HIV, with approximately 14% of the population affected by the virus (UNAIDS, 2019). Children and adolescents living with HIV are particularly vulnerable to neurocognitive deficits which result from neuroinvasion by the virus, together with the toxicity of combination antiretroviral therapies (cARTs) (Yuan & Kaul, 2019). These neurocognitive deficits typically manifest as difficulties in higher order executive functions, such as sustained attention and working memory (Milligan & Cockcroft, 2017).

These findings have a neuroanatomical basis since HIV preferentially affects neuronal networks associated with the fronto-striatal cortex, a key node of the central executive network (CEN), implicated with attention and executive functions (Yu et al., 2019). For example, Chang et al. (2001) investigated blood oxygen level dependency (BOLD) in HIV+ (n = 11; mean age = 41 years; SD = 4.8 years) and HIV unaffected controls (n = 11; mean age = 38 years; SD = 4.8 years) using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) during the execution of simple and complex attention tasks. In the simple attention reaction task, participants pressed a button in response to a number that appeared at random intervals on a screen, while the complex attention task required participants to respond only when the displayed number was twice as high as the preceding number. The complex attention task placed a greater cognitive load1 on participants, tapping into inhibitory control and working memory. For the simple attention task, the HIV group showed significantly greater BOLD activation in the inferior lateral prefrontal cortex, and parietal lobes compared to controls (p < 0.05). The increased neural activation in both the frontal and parietal regions for the HIV+ group during a simple attention task is proposed to reflect the brain’s compensatory response to neuroHIV.2 Increased BOLD activation within the prefrontal cortex is indicative of greater requirement for ‘top-down’ neuronal resources necessary to manipulate and maintain working memory during cognitively demanding tasks (Chang et al., 2001).

Building on their earlier findings, Chang et al. (2004) examined BOLD responses in the visual attention network among HIV+ (n = 18; mean age = 38.2 years; SD = 1.7 years) and HIV-unaffected participants (n = 18; M = 38 years; SD = 2.1 years). Consistent with Chang et al. (2001), HIV+ individuals showed significantly greater activation in the right prefrontal and parietal cortices under increasing cognitive load. This heightened neural activation was interpreted as compensatory recruitment linked to neuronal inefficiency associated with neuroHIV. These converging findings, alongside the limited cognitive benefits of cART, underscore the importance of developing non-pharmaceutical interventions to address HIV-related cognitive impairment.

Cognitive rehabilitation therapy (CRT), grounded in the principle of cortical neuroplasticity, holds potential to reverse cognitive decline in individuals with neuroHIV by promoting adaptive changes in the cerebral cortex (Crosson et al., 2017). Evidence supports CRT’s effectiveness in improving attention, working memory, and executive function among adolescents living with HIV (Ikekwere et al., 2021; Fraser & Cockcroft, 2020; Boivin et al., 2019). However, no studies to date have combined behavioural outcomes with objective neural measures, such as fNIRS, to assess CRT efficacy in this population.

The sole study (at the time of writing) to investigate brain plasticity in neuroHIV by pairing behavioural outcomes with objective brain measures is that of Chang et al. (2017). They paired working memory training (via CogMed) with fMRI in adult participants. At one-month follow-up, participants (HIV+: n = 34, mean age = 50.3 years; SD = 1.9. years; HIV-: n = 42, mean age = 52.6 years; SD = 1.7 years), who underwent 25 sessions of working memory training showed statistically significant decrease in BOLD activation when completing working memory tasks (i.e., the n-back task) compared to their performance prior to the training. The HIV group showed statistically significantly decreased BOLD activation in the right middle prefrontal gyrus when completing the 2-back working memory task, which also correlated with improved scores on untrained tasks, namely the Digit-Span Backward and the Spatial Span Forward, suggesting near transfer of skills to other measures of working memory. Six months later, both groups showed BOLD deactivation in the right medial prefrontal gyrus and primary motor area during the 1-back task. In addition, BOLD deactivation was correlated with significant improvement on the Digit-Span Forward working memory task, compared to pretest. The decreased BOLD-fMRI activation is proposed to indicate cortical reorganization associated with improved neural efficiency when completing working tasks of increased cognitive load (Chang et al., 2017).

Empirical research on the efficacy of CRT in HIV+ children and adolescents, particularly studies combining behavioural and brain-based measures, remains limited (Benki-Nugent & Boivin, 2019). Such interventions could be especially valuable in low- and middle-income contexts such as South Africa, where they may help alleviate the HIV associated mental health burden. This study thus (1) examined the impact of sustained attention training on behavioural performance (via the Stroop Colour Word Test) and (2) cortical hemodynamic responses (via fNIRS) in HIV+ adolescents receiving attention brain training. While prior research suggests brain training may enhance cognitive efficiency and reduce neural activation in relevant regions, no directional hypotheses were proposed for the study, due to the limited existing evidence from a single study with adult participants.

Research questions

Compared to controls receiving no attention training, do HIV+ participants receiving sustained attention training show significantly improved scores on the SCWT?

Do improvements on cognitive measures on the SCWT correlate with decreased hemodynamic responses in the prefrontal cortex, post training?

Methods

This project is part of a larger longitudinal pre-and-post-quasi experimental study to examine the effect of brain training within a cohort of children and adolescents living with HIV in South Africa (Zondo, et al., 2024b). Details of the experimental protocol can be found in (Zondo, et al., 2024a). The current investigation evaluated whether attention training is associated with improved neural efficiency and increased functional connectivity in the CEN, as measured by the SCWT and its correlation with hemodynamic responses in the prefrontal cortex as measured by fNIRS (via changes in oxygenated (HbO) and deoxygenated hemoglobin (Hb) concentration). A repeated measures design enabled us to collect behavioural and neuroimaging data pre- and post-training.

Participants

Purposive sampling was used to recruit 26 participants from three shelters caring for children living with HIV. All participants (age M = 17.28 years, SD = 1.94) were diagnosed with HIV3 and were on a course of cART. Exclusion criteria included (a) traumatic brain injury (b) central nervous system-related ailments (e.g., cerebral palsy, meningitis), or (c) learning difficulties. Written informed consent was obtained from the Directors of the shelters and, where possible, from guardians of the children. Assent was obtained from all participants. Ethical approval for this study was granted by Ethics Committee of the University of the Witwatersrand, South Africa [M211073].

Participants were randomly assigned to either the experimental (n = 13) or control group (n = 13) using Research Randomizer Software (Urbaniak & Plous, 2013). The control group followed a treatment-as-usual approach. One participant in the control group was excluded from the behavioural analysis because they did not respond to more than 38% of the trials and repeatedly pressed the same response key for both congruent and incongruent trials on the SCWT post-assessment. Table 1 summarises the sample characteristics. Chi-squared tests revealed no significant differences between the groups, by schooling (χ2 = 0.195, p = 0.658), there were, however, differences by sex (χ2 = 3.86, p = 0.05) and age (χ2 = 3.94, p = 0.05). Although all participants were on cART, significant differences were noted in terms of additional medication between the groups (χ2 = 6.01, p = 0.01) with three participants in the experimental group on a course of cART and psychotropic medication and one on ADHD medication.

Table 1. Demographics characteristics of participants and their comparisons.

Sample characteristicsTreatment groupControl groupχ2 p value
n = 13 n = 13
Sex (F/M)a5/89/43.860.05
Age Rangeb (0-13 years/14-18 years)3/108/53.930.05
Medication (Moodc/ADHD)2/10/06.190.01
School (Primary/Secondary)3/104/90.1950.658

a Female and Male categories for sex.

b Age categories were based on WHO age range suggestions for children and adolescents.

c Participants were receiving either Risperidol or Citalopram at low dosages.

Measures

Participants completed a battery of neurocognitive tests pre- and post-training as detailed in (Zondo, et al., 2024a). Assessments included a demographic questionnaire, the Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment Second Edition (NEPSY-II) and the Behaviour Rating Inventory for Executive Function (BRIEF).

Functional Near-Infrared Spectrometry (fNIRS) data was acquired using the NIRxSport2 (NIRx, Medical Technologies), a portable continuous wave fNIRS device, which was administered while participants completed the SCWT (described in Zondo, et al., 2024a). Specifically, the SWCT took the form of an fNIRS block design4 adapted from Schroeter et al. (2002). Before completing the computerised version of the SCWT, participants completed the pencil and paper version of the test and received feedback to improve test-wiseness. Lastly, this study followed recommendations for best practices in fNIRS research (Yücel et al., 2021) and the protocol used for data acquisition and montage is described in (Zondo, et al., 2024a).

Data analysis

Behavioural performance

Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS Statistics Version 27. First, the normality of the behavioural data was evaluated using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Then, the reaction time (in milliseconds) and accuracy (in total correct scores) data of the SCWT were analysed within groups and between groups (experimental vs control). Cohen’s d reported effects sizes.

fNIRS

Using guidelines for the analysis of repeated measures in fNIRS (NIRx Medical Technologies), standardized beta coefficients were first derived using Satori. For each channel, pre-processing filters were applied, followed by convolving each stimulus (congruent, incongruent) to a hemodynamic response model. General Linear Models were then applied with the measured fNIRS (oxyHb and Hb) signals as dependent variables, and the convolved functions as independent variables. The beta values of these models were used as estimates for hemodynamic responses for the brain regions of interest. The sign and magnitude of each beta coefficient provided an indicator of the direction (positive/negative) and intensity of concentration changes in HbO (i.e., cortical activity) for each condition. Once all beta values were estimated, we then pattened our analysis following Khoe et al. (2020). As such, beta values representing HbO response activation were recorded at pre-training, and these served as a baseline for post-training hemodynamic response comparison. Primary outcomes for HbO activation were, therefore, changes in HbO, at post-training, minus those at pre-training (ΔHbO = (Post-intervention HbO) − (Pre-intervention HbO)). Concentration changes (ΔHbO) were calculated for congruent and incongruent trials for both groups. Figure 1 provides an example of an event marker to calculate ΔHbO at pre- and post-test within Satori fNIRS.

7ab278c5-e9be-46ab-9fee-5cfa820ae098_figure1.gif

Figure 1. Event marker evaluating changes in oxygenated hemoglobin (ΔHbO), at pre- and post-test.

Note. Green bars under ‘parameter weights’ indicate congruent trials. Red bars indicate incongruent trials. Within the ‘Conditions’ panel, run1 indicates pretest HbO levels; run2, indicates post intervention HbO levels. For the above example, the congruent stimuli time course is indicated in green within the ‘Intervals’ (seconds) panel.

Changes in HbO (ΔHbO) within the experimental and control group were then represented by topographical maps of regions of the prefrontal cortex. Increase in brain hemodynamic response was represented in red. Increased oxygenated hemoglobin levels correlated with increased hemodynamic activation and cognitive effort. Light or dark blue represented hemodynamic deactivation and decreased brain activity. The Shapiro-Wilk Test evaluated normality of changes in oxygenated hemoglobin (ΔHbO), pre- and post-training. Differences in pre- and post-measures within each group were calculated using paired samples t-test. Between-group differences in ΔHbO were evaluated using an independent samples t-test. The Benjamini-Yekutieli method (Benjamini & Yekutieli, 2001) adjusted for multiple comparisons.

Results

Behavioural data

Within subjects group analyses

The reaction time and accuracy scores of the SCWT at pre- and post-test, for the groups are presented in Table 2. For the experimental group, within-group analyses indicated no significant differences in reaction times when correctly identifying congruent trials (M = -0.01; SD = 0.43, t (170) = - 0.45, p = 0.469, d = 0.43). Notably, although not significant, this group’s accuracy rates improved from 52% at pre-test to 61% post-test (p > 0.05). At pre-test, this group accurately identified 54.9% of incongruent trials, with a minimal improvement of 57% at post-test (p > 0.05). Notably, although there were increases in accuracy rates (%) at post-test, reaction times were significantly slower at post-test (M = 1.24), compared to pre-test (M = 1.09; p = 0.001, d = 0.6).

Table 2. Reaction time and accuracy % on the SCWT.

VariableExperimentalControl
MSDM SD
Reaction Time Sec (Congruent)
Pre1.090.30.970.02
Post1.030.21.170.07
Reaction Time Sec (Incongruent)
Pre1.240.41.030.2
Post1.270.51.150.03
Accuracy Rate %Accuracy Rate %
Accuracy % (Congruent)
Pre5247.8
Post61.439.1
Accuracy % (Incongruent)
Pre5445.1
Post5743.4

Within-groups analyses for the control group indicated an accuracy rate of 48% for congruent trials at pre-test and 39% at post-test. On average, participants indicated significantly slower reaction times (millisecond) at post-test (M = 1.37; SD = 0.81) compared to pre-test (M = 0.97) when correctly identifying congruent trials (M = -0.33; SD = 0.91, t (108) = -3.85, p = 0.011, d = 0.91). At pre-test, participants accurately identified 45.1% of incongruent trials and 43.4% at post-test (p > 0.05). On average, participants’ reaction time was significantly slower at post-test (M = 1.17; SD = 0.6) when identifying incongruent trials (M = 0.11; SD = 0.47, t (134) = -2.85, p = 0.05, d = 0.47).

Between subject group analyses

Between group analyses indicated no significant differences between the group’s reaction times at pre- and post-test, when correctly identifying congruent trials (U = 1175, p = 0.76). Similarly, no significant differences were found at pre- and post-test between the groups in terms of reaction times when accurately identifying incongruent trials (U = 1200, p = 0.065).

fNIRS Neuroimaging results

PFC brain activation: Pre & Post intervention: Congruent trial

Differences in brain activation are indicated by contrast t-statistics maps for HbO. We compared brain activation in the prefrontal cortex at pre- and post-test in relation to congruent and incongruent trials on the SCWT. Figure 2 indicates neuronal differences in ΔHbO between the groups during the congruent task. Although the control group indicated greater differences in HbO activation (ΔHbO), there were no significant differences (p > 0.05) between the groups on any of the optodes in the regions of interest.

7ab278c5-e9be-46ab-9fee-5cfa820ae098_figure2.gif

Figure 2. Changes in PFCA (ΔHbO) between the treatment and control groups on congruent trials.

Note. A. The experimental group indicated lesser hemodynamic changes at pre- and post-test (ΔHbO). B. The control group shows greater hemodynamic changes on pre- and post-test congruent trials (ΔHbO), but they were not statistically significant (p > 0.05). Concentration of colour blue denotes less HbO, and red, greater HbO.

Prefrontal cortex activation: Pre- and Post-test: Incongruent trials

Figure 3 shows differences in ΔHbO between the groups. Independent t-tests revealed that completing incongruent trials resulted in significantly lower hemodynamic responses in the left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPF) and left frontopolar Area5 in the experimental group compared to the control group (p < 0.05; see Table 3).

7ab278c5-e9be-46ab-9fee-5cfa820ae098_figure3.gif

Figure 3. Changes in PFCA (ΔHbO) between the experimental and control groups on incongruent trials.

Note. A. The experimental group indicated lesser hemodynamic changes at pre- and post-test (ΔHbO). B. The control group indicated greater hemodynamic changes in pre- and post-test incongruent trials (ΔHbO). Concentration of colour blue denotes less HbO, and red, greater HbO.

Table 3. Areas of significantly lower activation in the HIV treatment group in relation to completing incongruent trials.

ChannelOptode nameMNI positionBA Anatomical region
x y z
CH 2S1 - D346382445Right pars triangularis Broca’s Area
46Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
CH 7S3 - D5-46392645Left pars triangularis Broca’s Area
46Left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
CH 14S6 - D34846545Right pars triangularis Broca’s Area
46Right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
CH 18S7 - D7-1267010Left frontopolar area
11Left Orbitofrontal area
CH 19S8 - D5-4746645Left pars triangularis Broca’s area
46Left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
CH 20S8 - D7-2362239Left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
46Left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex
10Left frontopolar Area

Discussion

Disturbance in neural cytoarchitecture following HIV-infection is associated with decreased cognitive function, particularly in attention and executive functions, such as working memory (Hammond et al., 2019). Significantly, cART cannot reverse cognitive decline (Gonzalez et al., 2020). Functional neuroimaging studies suggest individuals living with HIV typically show lower BOLD activation in the CEN on simple undemanding tasks, but greater BOLD activation as cognitive load increases, in both the CEN and compensatory brain regions (i.e., the parietal regions) (Chang & Shukla, 2018). In individuals with neuroHIV, increased BOLD activation is typically a marker of neuronal inefficiency, as the cortex overcompensates and recruits associated neural networks in response to increasing cognitive demands (Chang et al., 2004, 2013).

With adults, there is evidence that working memory training may result in greater cognitive proficiency in related tasks and is associated with decreased BOLD activation, symbolizing neural efficiency and task proficiency (Chang et al., 2017). No empirical studies exist investigating changes in BOLD or hemodynamic responses in children or adolescents living in Sub-Saharan Africa, which continues to bear the burden of neuroHIV. Given this dearth, our study investigated whether customized attention remediation improved performance on the SCWT (a measure of attention and inhibitory control) and also whether any such cognitive outcomes were related to decreased hemodynamic response activation within the prefrontal cortex. Potential brain plasticity was investigated in our participants using optical neuroimaging in the form of fNIRS.

Although no significant behavioural differences were observed on the Stroop task, HIV+ participants who received sustained attention training showed modest improvements in congruent trial accuracy, while the control group’s performance declined. Incongruent trial accuracy remained largely unaffected by training. Notably, significant between-group differences emerged in hemodynamic responses during incongruent trials, suggesting neural effects of the intervention despite limited behavioural impact.

With reference to neural effects, we investigated whether improvements on the SCWT correlated with decreased hemodynamic responses in the prefrontal cortex following the attention training. While the control group showed increased hemodynamic response activation on ΔHbO compared to the experimental group on congruent trials, these changes were statistically insignificant. Concerning incongruent trials, there were significant differences in ΔHbO between the groups, with the control group showing increased hemodynamic responses, and the experimental group indicating attenuation in hemodynamic responses, suggesting greater neural efficiency in the latter group in dedicated brain areas. Defined cortical regions of hemodynamic attenuation were localised to the frontopolar/anterior prefrontal area, dorsolateral PFC, the orbitofrontal region, and the pars triangularis Broca’s Area, when completing incongruent trials of the SCWT.

The SCWT interference task typically places significant cognitive load on cortical processes, resulting in hyperactivation of cortical regions, including the dorsal, orbitofrontal, medial prefrontal and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (Banich, 2019). Our findings suggest that attention training decreases hemodynamic responses in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, a key network node with the CEN. Since the dorsolateral prefrontal network is connected to the orbitofrontal cortex through association fibres, the concomitant decrease in hemodynamic responses in these regions may suggest that cognitive training could strengthen participants’ ability to inhibit automatic responses and enable better monitoring and adjustment of their response strategies (which improved their accuracy on incongruent trials of the SCWT) (Spielberg et al., 2015).

The findings from our study corroborate those of Chang et al. (2017), who observed decreased BOLD activation in the dorsal and lateral prefrontal cortices following working memory intervention. In their study, decreased BOLD activation was accompanied by improved performance on the 2-back working memory task. Where our findings differ from those by Chang et al. (2017), is that we did not find concomitant increased scores on the SCWT at post-test.

Notwithstanding the absence of significant cognitive test score improvements following the attention training, decreased cortical activation in the frontopolar area, as observed in the experimental group, is an interesting finding. This area is implicated in maintaining alertness and subsequent retrieval of stored information (Hogeveen et al., 2022). Due to projections of the frontopolar region to the anterior cingulate cortex, the latter which is implicated in neuroHIV (Israel et al., 2019; Toich et al., 2017), decreased hemodynamic activation in Area 10 may suggests improved neural efficiency and cortical proficiency. Moreover, attenuation in hemodynamic responses may indicate reduced involvement of the ‘top-down attention network’ (CEN) during demanding cognitive tasks such as the SCWT (Banich, 2019).

Importantly, we found decreased hemodynamic responses in the left pars triangularis (Broca’s Area) and the orbitofrontal area. Broca’s area is activated during the completion of the SCWT, especially in incongruent trials (Wallentin et al., 2015). To this end, the incongruent trial of the SCWT places significant cognitive load on language processes that require identifying the ink colour of the stimulus, while inhibiting any conflict that arises with the actual word name (which is also a colour). In addition to correctly identifying incongruent stimuli, participants manipulated visual data (colour/word) and employed inhibitory control, in order not to associate a colour with a word (colour name), thus engaging the orbitofrontal cortex (Rolls et al., 1996). Thus, a decrease in hemodynamic activation in this region suggests decreased requirement for ‘top-down’ attention processing, which was found following the training.

Although the experimental group showed cortical-level changes in hemodynamic activity, these neural effects were not accompanied by significant improvements in SCWT accuracy or reaction times compared to controls. This dissociation between neural and behavioural outcomes is consistent with prior findings, such as those reported in a systematic review by Brooks et al. (2020), which noted that neural adaptations—including changes in BOLD activation and functional connectivity—often precede observable cognitive gains. Such delays may be influenced by factors like training duration, neural plasticity, and intervention intensity. These findings suggest that early neural changes may reflect emerging plasticity, with behavioural improvements potentially requiring extended intervention periods to manifest. Several empirical studies support this view (Powell & Redish, 2016; Tremblay et al., 1998).

Conclusions

This appears to be the first study investigating hemodynamic responses related to attention training in pediatric neuroHIV. Despite promising findings—namely, reduced prefrontal hemodynamic activation post-training and improved SCWT performance on incongruent trials—several limitations must be acknowledged. Due to the nature of the sample (vulnerable children living with HIV), the study experienced high attrition, which led to a reduced sample size, limiting statistical power, and pre-test group differences in age and sex may have influenced outcomes. Additionally, important moderators such as HIV genotype, age of cART initiation, and individual neurobiological variability were not assessed (Brew & Garber, 2018). Moreover, we only investigated neural changes one month after the cognitive training and further post-test follow ups would have added valuable data about whether any effects were sustained over time. The fNIRS protocol was also limited in spatial coverage (Extended Data - Supplementary File) and lacked short-separation channels to control for physiological noise (Pfeifer et al., 2018). Nonetheless, the observed neural attenuation suggests emerging neuroplasticity, supporting the potential of attention training—alongside cART—to enhance cognitive outcomes in children and adolescents affected by neuroHIV.

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 29 Sep 2025
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Zondo S, Cockcroft K, da Silva Ferreira Barreto C and Ferreira Correia A. Shining Light into Adolescent HIV Neuroplasticity: A Study of the Prefrontal Cortex Using Functional Near Infrared Spectrometry [version 1; peer review: awaiting peer review]. F1000Research 2025, 14:1000 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.169799.1)
NOTE: If applicable, it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status:
AWAITING PEER REVIEW
AWAITING PEER REVIEW
?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 29 Sep 2025
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.