Keywords
coral reefs, fish, colours, camouflage, signal-transmission, evolution, conspicuousness
coral reefs, fish, colours, camouflage, signal-transmission, evolution, conspicuousness
Being able to hide in plain sight is a major selective pressure for both prey and predator species1,2. Traits increasing the ability of individuals to camouflage in the environment have likely been under strong selection since vision emerged, having guided to a great extent the evolution of visual displays in the animal world. It is in this context that the dazzling colouration of fish inhabiting coral reefs and other tropical bodies of water has puzzled scientists since the formulation of natural selection theory3–9. This flamboyance of colour patterns seems not only to disregard any pressure for blending in with the environment, but in fact suggests the opposite purpose: to make an individual stand out as much as possible, competing for attention.
Alfred Russel Wallace, co-proponent of the natural selection theory, put forward a hypothesis that relied instead on the existence of a camouflage purpose of those colours and patterns, whereby “brilliantly-coloured fishes from warm seas are many of them well concealed when surrounded by the brilliant sea-weeds, corals, sea-anemones, and other marine animals, which make the sea-bottom sometimes resemble a fantastic flower-garden”3. Even though camouflage may also work by disrupting, or breaking up, contrasting patterns that can make a prey/predator easily recognizable1,10, the fact is that most fish inhabiting coral reefs can be easily seen to the point that, in fact, 1973 Nobel Prize winner, Konrad Lorenz, proposed a hypothesis which is based in a total denial of disguise function in that context. Instead, Lorenz suggested these dazzling colour patterns would be a robust means of species-recognition in the highly diverse and multi-niche environment of coral reefs, where such distinct signalling patterns would be needed to prevent aggression among non-competitor species4. However, many of the colourful fish found in corals are not necessarily aggressive or territorial11. Therefore, to date no hypothesis has in fact proven resilient to existing empirical data and this evolutionary puzzle remains at large4–9.
Here I present the “Hyper-Visible World” hypothesis, which proposes that coral reef habitats impose special conditions on the evolution of body-colour communication for mobile fish – that is, fish that roam across coral reef formations-leading to an impossibility of the use of camouflage while promoting the selective forces that benefit from conspicuousness. The special conditions are: 1) the high clarity of water during daylight hours, and 2) the unpredictable pattern/visual complexity of the coral habitat itself. These conditions negate the possibility of camouflage for most mobile animals in such habitats (some species can change the body coloration in real time as they roam over different backgrounds, but this is a highly sophisticated and demanding feature restricted to a few species like some octopuses). Because signalling patterns evolve as a trade-off between predation and other selective pressures12, when predation under varying degrees of visual conspicuousness is similarly efficient, other selective pressures for communication that benefit from conspicuousness (e.g. sexual or warning signalling) can evolve without the constraints imposed by the need for camouflage.
The hypothesis may be also understood within a signal transmission framework, where the visual conspicuousness of an individual represents the signal. As such, in coral reefs, the intensity of the signal conveyed by mobile fish cannot be reduced as to lessen predatory pressures. The exceptionally good environment for signal transmission (clear waters) and the unpredictability of the “background noise” (diverse coral reef) for a dislocating individual, makes the reduction of signal-to-noise ratio exceptionally difficult. In fact, the Hyper-Visible World hypothesis lays out a specific and falsifiable (sensu Popper13) prediction: other traits being equal, roaming fish with any degree of visual prominence will endure equivalent predatory pressure (or success) in coral reefs, but not when swimming against a predictable background.
It is important to highlight spatiotemporal dynamics14 involved in this theory: the degree of mobility of fish in the geography of coral reef habitats plays a pivotal role in the predictability of the background, and hence in the evolution of camouflage. If a fish roams against a variety of backdrops, the likelihood of effective camouflage is close to null. If, on the other hand, a fish spends most of his time in one location, natural selection will favour pigmentation and morphologies that match that predictable substrate (be it a coral species, type of rock or sand). Interestingly, because visual acuity is so high in the transparent waters of coral reefs, the need to “deceive with perfection” is also exceptionally high, leading to the “hyper-naturalism” found in the camouflage patterns of fish like pygmy sea horse or anglerfish, which is more typical of terrestrial environments (where visibility is also usually excellent) than of other marine habitats.
Selective pressures driving the evolution of colour patterns (but that usually come secondary to camouflage) benefit precisely from those conditions that are adverse to concealment. Those selective pressures range from hostile to friendly signalling. Among conspecifics, for example, signals range from those communicating willingness to engage in dispute over resources to stressing bonding forces for school formation and sexual attraction. In interspecific interactions, signals may range from warnings of retaliatory weaponry (e.g. aposematism by poisonous fishes) to the marketing of services (e.g. special colours and approaching behaviours of cleaner fishes4,15). However, while selective pressures for conspicuousness are favoured by the transparency of the medium, they are hampered by the complex and colour-rich background of the coral reef - hence the pressure for the “hyper-unnatural” (i.e. not often found in nature) colour patterns of many reef fish.
Although the Hyper-Visible World hypothesis relies on the notion that camouflage in coral reefs is not an option for many mobile species, selection for camouflage can be also relaxed in other contexts. One such case is that of non-predatory species endowed with effective defence mechanisms against predators. The hummingbirds’ speed or the nut-cracking beaks of parrots probably did not evolve as defences against predation, but are effective in that sense, freeing those animals from the need to visually blend in with their surroundings. Similarly to what is found in coral reefs, other evolutionary pressures for colouration could then take over.
A Hyper-Visible World, we speculate, can also help drive biological diversity. Signalling in high visual resolution can promote the genesis of new species through sensory-drive12,16, a process whereby subtle changes in either colour patterns or in sensory/cognitive biases for attraction to those patterns can lead to the reproductive isolation of part of a population. In this sense, the high resolution of signals coupled with the high productivity of coral reefs might account for the high rates of sympatric speciation observed in these habitats.
Justin Marshall, a specialist in the study of colour vision observed that it “is almost inconceivable for only one evolutionary force to be behind the colours of such a diverse assemblage”6. Indeed, should the Hyper-Visible World hypothesis prove accurate, it is paradoxically the very elimination of only one evolutionary force (camouflage) that sets the artistic boldness of several other pressures free for drawing the magnificent mosaic of colours and shapes found in these marine ecological wonders.
I am grateful to Gil Rosenthal, for his comments on an earlier version on this paper, and to Cynthia Schuck-Paim, Nigel Pearn and Veronique Vicera for their comments and editorial content revisions. This paper is dedicated to the dear friends of the memorable 2014 sailing trip, the occasion when I formulated this theory while diving in the outstanding Caribbean waters.
Views | Downloads | |
---|---|---|
F1000Research | - | - |
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
|
- | - |
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Invited Reviewers | |||
---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | |
Version 2 (revision) 22 Mar 16 |
read | read | |
Version 1 12 May 15 |
read |
Provide sufficient details of any financial or non-financial competing interests to enable users to assess whether your comments might lead a reasonable person to question your impartiality. Consider the following examples, but note that this is not an exhaustive list:
Sign up for content alerts and receive a weekly or monthly email with all newly published articles
Already registered? Sign in
The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.
You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.
You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.
If your email address is registered with us, we will email you instructions to reset your password.
If you think you should have received this email but it has not arrived, please check your spam filters and/or contact for further assistance.
Thank you very much for your encouraging note and your very relevant feedback.
Below are my responses to your comments:
B#1 "I've been reading your piece with a particular attention. I ... Continue reading Dear Ben,
Thank you very much for your encouraging note and your very relevant feedback.
Below are my responses to your comments:
B#1 "I've been reading your piece with a particular attention. I really liked your proposition. I just identified one possible caveat, that you should take into account from my point of view. As you underlined: "If, on the other hand, a fish spends most of his time in one location, natural selection will favour pigmentation and morphologies that match that predictable substrate (be it a coral species, type of rock or sand)." However their are some examples of fish living in very high degree of vicinity with their home habitat that display very different colour pattern to their predictable substrate."
Thank you for pointing this out. You are indeed correct regarding my imprecision in the sentence you have referenced. I am changing the word "will" to "can" in the text to emphasize that the conditions allow for the possibility of camouflage, rather than an "imposition" for camouflage. But, of course, as you accurately pointed out, fish, particularly clownfish, don't follow this "rule." This is exactly the case with macaws and hummingbirds, which I did cite, and in which the circumstance is not that the organism cannot camouflage, but it does not need to camouflage. It has the protection of sea anemones, so why bother when it can utilize the canvas of the body for other purposes.
I also added a figure in this version that attempts to highlight the difference between these two important circumstances which are unfavourable to camouflage mechanisms ("adverse conditions for camouflage" found among roaming fish in coral reefs and "no need for camouflage" found among macaws and hummingbirds)
So, going back to your clownfish example, because in this particular case, they are so well shielded against predation by their biological alliance with poisonous anemone, their striking coloration is a product of residing the "carefree world" region above, rather than the "hyper-visible world." The clownfish develop conspicuousness not out of impossibility (the background where it spends most of its time -the anemone- is quite predictable), but out of disdain for camouflage. This is a descriptive example I hadn’t thought about previously, and so I am adding it to the edited version.
As for your comments observed during your own experience in Fernando de Noronha (lucky you!), I would say that, despite the coral reefs not being as colourful as in other places, the "hyper-visible" conditions are still present: the water is very transparent and the background is still very unpredictable for those species that roam near the rocks/corals, so therefore, camouflage is not an option for them. You are indeed correct in your argument that comparative studies need to take into account all conditions in these different habitats (micro and macro) and can test this (as well as alternative) theories.
Thanks once again for your much appreciated input.
Best,
Wladimir
Thank you very much for your encouraging note and your very relevant feedback.
Below are my responses to your comments:
B#1 "I've been reading your piece with a particular attention. I really liked your proposition. I just identified one possible caveat, that you should take into account from my point of view. As you underlined: "If, on the other hand, a fish spends most of his time in one location, natural selection will favour pigmentation and morphologies that match that predictable substrate (be it a coral species, type of rock or sand)." However their are some examples of fish living in very high degree of vicinity with their home habitat that display very different colour pattern to their predictable substrate."
Thank you for pointing this out. You are indeed correct regarding my imprecision in the sentence you have referenced. I am changing the word "will" to "can" in the text to emphasize that the conditions allow for the possibility of camouflage, rather than an "imposition" for camouflage. But, of course, as you accurately pointed out, fish, particularly clownfish, don't follow this "rule." This is exactly the case with macaws and hummingbirds, which I did cite, and in which the circumstance is not that the organism cannot camouflage, but it does not need to camouflage. It has the protection of sea anemones, so why bother when it can utilize the canvas of the body for other purposes.
I also added a figure in this version that attempts to highlight the difference between these two important circumstances which are unfavourable to camouflage mechanisms ("adverse conditions for camouflage" found among roaming fish in coral reefs and "no need for camouflage" found among macaws and hummingbirds)
So, going back to your clownfish example, because in this particular case, they are so well shielded against predation by their biological alliance with poisonous anemone, their striking coloration is a product of residing the "carefree world" region above, rather than the "hyper-visible world." The clownfish develop conspicuousness not out of impossibility (the background where it spends most of its time -the anemone- is quite predictable), but out of disdain for camouflage. This is a descriptive example I hadn’t thought about previously, and so I am adding it to the edited version.
As for your comments observed during your own experience in Fernando de Noronha (lucky you!), I would say that, despite the coral reefs not being as colourful as in other places, the "hyper-visible" conditions are still present: the water is very transparent and the background is still very unpredictable for those species that roam near the rocks/corals, so therefore, camouflage is not an option for them. You are indeed correct in your argument that comparative studies need to take into account all conditions in these different habitats (micro and macro) and can test this (as well as alternative) theories.
Thanks once again for your much appreciated input.
Best,
Wladimir