ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Research Note
Revised

Sensorimotor learning deficits observed in children with sports-related concussion

[version 2; peer review: 2 approved with reservations]
PUBLISHED 23 Jul 2015
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

Abstract

Mild traumatic brain injury, commonly known as concussion, occurs frequently in children while playing sports. Whereas the negative impact of sport-related concussions on cognitive function is well known, the effects of concussions on sensorimotor function, especially in children, remain largely unknown. In this observational/case study, we investigated the association between neurocognitive function, assessed using a trail making test, and sensorimotor function, assessed using a visuomotor adaptation task, in three children who suffered from a concussion while playing sports. For the trail making test, children drew lines to connect numbers and letters alternately on a page. For the visuomotor adaptation task, children performed targeted reaching movements repeatedly under a novel visuomotor condition in which the visual display of reaching movements was rotated 30 degrees about the start position. Their neurocognitive and visuomotor performances were compared to those obtained from three children without a concussion. Results showed that only one of the three concussed children showed a score from the trail making test that was worse than the scores obtained from control subjects. However, the pattern of visuomotor adaptation observed in the concussed children was different from that observed in the control subjects in several ways. These data indicate that the concussed children may have some deficits in terms of their sensorimotor function as compared to the control subjects. Our findings provide preliminary data that suggest that children with concussion may have sensorimotor impairments even when they do not seem to have neurocognitive impairments.

Keywords

Mild Traumatic Brain Injury, Motor Learning, Visuomotor Adaptation, Trail Making Test

Revised Amendments from Version 1

In this version, we emphasized the nature of this article (i.e., observational/qualitative/case study) in the introduction section, and provided additional information on the subjects in the methods section. In the discussion section, we included additional statements regarding a limitation of our study that was identified by the reviewer, which is that age and sex were not matched perfectly between the patients and the controls.

See the authors' detailed response to the review by Lauren Sergio

Introduction

Pediatric concussions have been shown to negatively affect neurocognitive function, including poor attention span, impaired memory and learning difficulties13. However, the association between pediatric concussion and sensorimotor function has not been investigated. Understanding motor learning impairment following sport-related concussion in children may be critical in guiding safe return to play. In this short observational/case study, we investigated the pattern of adaptation to a novel visuomotor condition in three children who suffered a concussion while playing sports, and compared their adaptation patterns to those obtained from three children without a concussion. A research paradigm in which individuals adapt to a novel visuomotor condition during reaching movements has been employed extensively in the neuroscience community to understand the neural processes that underlie motor learning in both adults4,5 and children6,7.

We also examined neurocognitive function of both the concussed children and the control children using an assessment technique called the trail making test (TMT). TMT (part B) is a traditional paper assessment that times subjects as they draw lines to connect numbers and letters alternately on a page. TMT is a validated assessment of concussion, and tests cognitive demands that are also important for sports, including psychomotor processing, visual motor/spatial abilities and mental flexibility8,9.

The purpose of this observational/case study was to determine qualitatively whether concussed children with neurocognitive impairments, as indicated by the TMT scores, would also demonstrate sensorimotor deficits, as indicated by the visuomotor adaptation patterns.

Methods

Three children (15 years old, one male (cc3)), who presented to the Emergency Department at the Children’s Hospital of Wisconsin within 24 hours from the time of injury and who received a diagnosis of concussion (Glasgow Coma Scale ≥ 14), participated in this study. None of the three children had a concussion previously. They visited the Neuromechanics Laboratories at the University of Wisconsin-Milwaukee (UWM) 5~8 days following the concussion. Their symptoms were not assessed on the day they visited UWM, although one of the patients (cc1) was not able to speak or walk normally at that time. Three children (12 (cc1), 14 (cc3) and 17 (cc2) years old, all males), who were recruited from the Milwaukee Metropolitan area, served as controls. Selection criteria for subjects were the same for both patients and controls (except their concussion status), which were: subject was 10–17 years of age, regularly participated in an athletic activity, was English-speaking, was right handed, and had no neurological disease or peripheral disorder affecting movement of the right arm. All subjects were recruited and tested in May 2015.

Upon arrival at UWM, the subjects were first administered with the TMT8,9. Following that, they participated in the visuomotor adaptation experiment in which they performed rapid reaching movements from a start circle to a target repeatedly under a normal visuomotor condition (baseline) first, then under a novel visuomotor condition (adaptation). The baseline session (40 trials) was provided for the subjects to become familiarized with the general reaching task with unperturbed visual feedback. In the adaptation session (80 trials), the visual display of reaching movements was rotated 30 degrees counterclockwise about the start circle, such that a hand movement made in the “12 o’clock” direction resulted in a cursor movement made in the “11 o’clock” direction. Continuous visual feedback (in the form of a cursor) was provided throughout the movement in both sessions. A robotic exoskeleton called KINARM (BKIN Technologies Ltd, Kingston, ON, Canada) was used to provide the visuomotor rotation during the experiment and also to collect movement data. The 2-D position of arm segments was sampled at 1,000Hz, low-pass filtered at 15Hz, and differentiated to yield resultant velocity values. Data were processed and analyzed using MATLAB (The Mathworks Inc., Natick, MA) and SPSS.

To examine performance accuracy, we calculated direction error (DE), which was the angular difference between a vector from the start circle to the target and another vector from the hand position at movement start to that at peak arm velocity. Using the direction error data, we obtained the following measures for each subject: (1) DE at trial 1 in the adaptation session; (2) the first block of DE (i.e., mean of five consecutive trials) in the adaptation session that is not statistically different from the last block of DE in the baseline session; and (3) the rate of performance change during the adaptation session. To obtain the second measure, a priori pairwise comparisons, using t-tests, were made between DE at block 8 from the baseline session and DE at each of the 16 blocks from the adaptation session (starting from block 1). The alpha level was set at .05. To obtain the third measure, a line of approximation was constructed by fitting a logarithmic regression line to the adaptation data, and the slope value was used.

Ethics

All subjects and their parents signed the assent/consent forms approved by the Institutional Review Board of UWM (IRB# 15.172).

Sample size

Because of the nature of the present study (i.e., observational/qualitative/case study of concussed children), we only tried to recruit a small number of concussed children. We recruited all our subjects (three concussed children and three controls who met our selection criteria) within a 10-day window.

Results

Figure 1c illustrates neurocognitive data from all subjects, indicated by TMT scores. One concussed child (cc1) showed some cognitive slowing on visit 1, as compared with the controls, while the others did not.

The overall pattern of visuomotor adaptation, illustrated in Figure 1a and b, was somewhat similar between the patients and the controls. In fact, the patients’ baseline performances do not appear to be worse than those of the controls. However, a close examination of the adaptation data revealed the following differences:

  • 1. DE at trial 1 of the adaptation session was somewhat larger for the patients than the controls; and within-group variability for this measure was also larger for the patients (Figure 1d).

  • 2. The first block of DE in the adaptation session that was not statistically different from the last block of DE in the baseline session was observed much later in two of the patients than in the controls; and within-group variability for this measure was much larger for the patients (Figure 1b, e).

  • 3. As indicated by the rate of performance change, it took longer for the patients than the controls to adapt to the visuomotor rotation; and within-group variability for this measure was slightly larger for the patients (Figure 1f).

070880da-c74a-484a-8021-80f7821f046d_figure1.gif

Figure 1.

(a) Improvements in performance across trials during baseline and adaptation sessions. Upper panel depicts data from 3 controls, lower panel data from 3 concussed children. (b) Improvements in performance across blocks during the baseline (last block only) and adaptation sessions. Numbers under arrows indicate first block of DE that was not statistically different from last block of DE from the baseline phase. (c) TMT scores for every subject. (d) DE at trial 1 during the adaptation session. (e) First block of DE in the adaptation session that was not different from last block of DE in the baseline session. (f) Rate of performance change in the adaptation session. Vertical boxes in red (cf) indicate variability within each subject group.

Dataset 1.Kinematic data from visuomotor adaptation task.
This SPSS file contains kinematic data from 3 subjects in each of the two groups (CC indicating concussed children, CTL controls). Subjects (ss) experienced two experimental sessions (1 indicating baseline, 2 visuomotor adaptation condition). To compute mean direction errors (dir_err) across 5 consecutive trials, the numbers shown in column ‘trial’ were converted into the numbers shown in column ‘block 2’. The numbers shown in column ‘block 2’ were used to make statistical comparisons between mean direction error from the last block of baseline session and that from each of 16 blocks of adaptation session within each subject. Rot_ang indicates the degree to which the visual display was rotated13.

Discussion

TMT is a validated neurocognitive assessment of pediatric concussion8,9. The average time to complete the TMT part B is 75 seconds in neurologically intact individuals; if the time is longer than 273 seconds, it is considered deficient10,11. One concussed child who participated in our study completed the TMT part B in 98 seconds; and the other two children completed it in less than 50 seconds. According to the TMT scores, thus, one may conclude that all of the children who had a concussion 5 to 8 days prior to their participation in this study did not seem to have neurocognitive impairments.

The patterns of visuomotor adaptation observed in the patients, however, appear to be somewhat different from those observed in the controls. Specifically, the first block of DE in the adaptation session that was significantly different from the last block of DE of the baseline session occurred later in the patients than in the controls; and the rate of performance change was higher (i.e., slower adaptation) in the patients as well. These data indicate that the patients had more difficulty than the controls while adapting to the novel visuomotor rotation, which points to the possibility of sensorimotor learning deficits in the concussed children.

The present study has several limitations. First, all the children tested in this study were recruited in the Milwaukee Metropolitan area, which raises a possibility that they may not be the best representative of all children of the same age group. Second, we did not collect any information regarding our subjects’ demographic and social statuses, which could be considered as potential confounding characteristics. Third, age and sex were not matched perfectly between the patients and the controls tested in the present study. However, we are not aware of any findings that indicate significant differences between boys and girls within the age range of 12–17 years with regard to their visuomotor adaptation capabilities. In addition, it has been shown that during targeted reaching movements under a visuomotor rotation condition, visuomotor representations of children who were 6 or 8 years old differed from those of adults, although those of 11 year-old children did not12. Our data do not seem to indicate sex-related differences either, in that the adaptation pattern of cc2 (15 year-old female) was very similar to that of ctls 1 and 3 (12 and 14 year-old males, respectively). Thus, while the potential effects of age- and sex-related differences between the patients and the controls cannot be completely excluded, it seems unlikely that our results were substantially influenced by such effects. Finally, we did not conduct statistical analyses to quantify the differences between the children with concussion and those without concussion, because the statistical results would not be very meaningful given the small sample size. Nevertheless, our data exhibited qualitative differences between the two groups of children that suggest sensorimotor deficits in concussed children. Further investigation is warranted to demonstrate quantitative differences between children with and children without concussion by utilizing a larger sample size.

In conclusion, the results from this study provide preliminary data indicating that children with concussion may have sensorimotor impairments even when they do not seem to have neurocognitive impairments. Given the importance of children’s ability to rapidly adapt to the dynamic environments and task requirements throughout a sport, our findings suggest that examination of sensorimotor function, in addition to that of neurocognitive function, may provide valuable information when determining whether children are ready to return to play sports or not.

Data availability

F1000Research: Dataset 1. Kinematic data from visuomotor adaptation task, 10.5256/f1000research.6575.d4898413

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 2
VERSION 2 PUBLISHED 05 Jun 2015
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Wang J, Thomas D and Cho YI. Sensorimotor learning deficits observed in children with sports-related concussion [version 2; peer review: 2 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2015, 4:143 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.6575.2)
NOTE: If applicable, it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 2
VERSION 2
PUBLISHED 23 Jul 2015
Revised
Views
5
Cite
Reviewer Report 12 Jan 2016
Lauren Sergio, Center for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, ON, USA 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 5
The authors now make it clear that this is more of a case study report. Again, this is an interesting approach that will likely prove useful to the field. I continue to look forward to a full study confirming these preliminary ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Sergio L. Reviewer Report For: Sensorimotor learning deficits observed in children with sports-related concussion [version 2; peer review: 2 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2015, 4:143 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.7366.r9623)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Views
9
Cite
Reviewer Report 02 Sep 2015
Liana Brown, Department of Psychology, Trent University, Peterborough, ON, Canada 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 9
This paper asks a very interesting question and uses a well-established motor learning paradigm in an attempt to answer it. I do, however, have concerns about the data and how they are interpreted.
  1. The low number of Ps is problematic. There
... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Brown L. Reviewer Report For: Sensorimotor learning deficits observed in children with sports-related concussion [version 2; peer review: 2 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2015, 4:143 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.7366.r9652)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 05 Jun 2015
Views
34
Cite
Reviewer Report 24 Jun 2015
Lauren Sergio, Center for Vision Research, York University, Toronto, ON, USA 
Not Approved
VIEWS 34
This study describes a psychophysical experiment in which three children who had recently experienced a concussion were asked to perform a visuomotor adaptation. Their performance was compared to three children with no concussion history. While the motivation for the study ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Sergio L. Reviewer Report For: Sensorimotor learning deficits observed in children with sports-related concussion [version 2; peer review: 2 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2015, 4:143 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.7060.r8919)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 23 Jul 2015
    Jinsung Wang, Department of Kinesiology, University of Wisconsin- Milwaukee, Milwaukee, 53201, USA
    23 Jul 2015
    Author Response
    Thank you for your review. In our responses below, we have attempted to address each concern raised by you.
    • Introduction: “However, the association between pediatric concussion and sensorimotor function has not
    ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 23 Jul 2015
    Jinsung Wang, Department of Kinesiology, University of Wisconsin- Milwaukee, Milwaukee, 53201, USA
    23 Jul 2015
    Author Response
    Thank you for your review. In our responses below, we have attempted to address each concern raised by you.
    • Introduction: “However, the association between pediatric concussion and sensorimotor function has not
    ... Continue reading

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 2
VERSION 2 PUBLISHED 05 Jun 2015
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.