Keywords
Cohort studies, socioeconomic status, covariates
Cohort studies, socioeconomic status, covariates
The paper has been revised, partly based on edits from Dr Vedoy who reviewed the paper. I have added the words “that investigate the relationship between diet and disease” to the title. I have also made an addition to the Discussion in the section where I discuss the relative importance of income and education in relation to how SES might affect risk of disease. The following has been added: “It is also possible that the reverse may be true. Education is stable throughout adulthood and provides individuals with certain abilities related to learning and thinking which are important in matters of health.”
See the author's detailed response to the review by Tord Finne Vedøy
Socioeconomic status (SES) is related to diet and health. It is well established that affluent people have superior health compared to poorer people1. A major part of the explanation for this is that people with high SES generally follow a healthier lifestyle1. Of particular note, many studies have reported that people with a higher SES consume a more nutritious diet2. This pattern has been consistently reported in the USA, Canada, and many European countries, and is seen with different indicators of SES – education, income, and occupation.
This suggests that income, or other indicators of SES, may be a relevant factor in cohort studies. It follows, therefore, that failure to include SES in multivariate analysis of findings from cohort studies may be a source of error due to residual confounding. The potential importance of this was shown by an analysis of data from the British Women’s Heart and Health Study. Adjusting for a range of factors that indicate SES, both those in childhood and adulthood, attenuated the relationship between the plasma concentration of vitamins C and E and risk of coronary heart disease (CHD) in adults aged over 60 years3,4.
The analysis described here was carried out in order to investigate the extent of this potential source of error.
An analysis was carried out of papers that reported the findings of cohort studies. The main inclusion criteria were, first, the papers were published in journals in the year 2000 or later, and, second, they reported findings on the relationship between dietary intake and health-related outcomes, such as body weight or disease. The papers were found using two main search strategies: (1) they were cited in various meta-analyses; and (2) by searching journals that cover the areas of nutrition, health, and medicine but with the search restricted to issues published in late 2013. A list of all papers used in this study is included in the Supplementary material.
Each paper was studied and key information was extracted. In particular, a record was made indicating whether the risk ratios were adjusted for factors that indicate SES, such as education and income. Where available, information was also extracted that reported associations between SES and dietary variables (e.g., whether study subjects with more education consumed more fish). In addition, for comparative purposes a record was kept of whether alcohol, physical activity, and hypertension were included as covariates.
Many cases were found where two or more papers were based on the same cohort study. In those cases each paper was evaluated. However, only one of the papers was included here unless, with respect to the covariates that are the focus of this paper, the papers used different covariates in the multivariate analysis. Papers were also included if they contained relevant information on the association between SES and dietary variables. After these exclusions and inclusions 76 papers from 66 cohort studies were included in the final analysis. Ten were published in the years 2000 to 2003, 15 during 2004 to 2007, 27 during 2008 to 2012, and 24 during 2013. The study is therefore based on a convenience sample of cohort studies rather than a systematic review. Nevertheless, because of the large number of cohort studies included, the findings are likely to be representative of cohort studies published in recent years.
The findings reported here are based on 66 cohort studies of which 52 were carried out on adults and 14 on children (age <18 years). They were carried out in the USA (30), Europe (28), Asia (5), and Canada (3).
The analysis included 76 papers. They covered a wide variety of diseases/health-related outcomes, including body weight or another measure of adiposity (25), cancer (21), type 2 diabetes (5), and all-cause mortality (16). Cardiovascular disease was studied in 33 papers, of which 19 looked at all forms of CVD combined, 13 at CHD, one at both CVD and CHD, and two at stroke. These 76 papers covered many aspects of the diet, including sugar-sweetened beverages (18), diet patterns (14), multivitamin supplements (8), fish (7), milk (7), sodium (6), and meat (4). The great majority of the papers covered only one dietary variable; the main exception was studies on beverages where different beverages were often included in the same paper. Physical activity was included as a covariate in 56 of the 76 papers (73.7%), alcohol in 43 of the 61 papers on adults (70.5%), and hypertension or blood pressure in 21 of the 33 papers on CVD (63.6%). At least one measure of SES was included as a covariate in 50 of the 76 papers (65.8%). Education was by far the most common (60.5%), followed by income (14.4%) and social class (2.6%). Several papers (11.8%) included more than one measure of SES.
Papers based on studies of children used a measure of SES with a similar frequency to those on adults. Parental SES was used in those studies.
Many of the papers reviewed here reported on the association between SES and diet. In general, people with more education consumed a diet that is associated with better health. This includes more fish5–7, less meat8,9, as well as a healthier overall dietary pattern10–13. In addition, some papers reported that those with more education consume more sugar-sweetened beverages14,15, more fruit juice14,16, and more sodium17,18. A particularly clear trend is the association between education and consumption of multivitamin supplements19–24.
A small number of the papers reported on the association between income and diet. Consistent with the above findings higher income is associated with eating more fish6,7 and a healthier overall dietary pattern12.
The methodology used here has strengths and weaknesses. The papers analyzed were mostly published in the last few years (51 of the 77 papers appeared between 2008 and 2014 while the remainder appeared between 2000 and 2008). They cover a wide range of dietary components and outcomes. Sixty-four of the 66 cohort studies were carried out in the USA, Canada, Europe, or Japan. The findings are therefore likely to be representative of cohort studies published in recent years that investigated the relationship between diet and risk of a wide variety of diseases and health-related outcomes. However, as the papers analyzed were randomly selected, there may be a degree of selection bias.
Approximately two-thirds of the papers (65.8%) used at least one measure of SES as a covariate. The most commonly used one was education (used in 60.5% of papers), followed by income (14.4%) and social class (2.6%). Some papers (11.8%) used more than one measure of SES. The frequency of inclusion of SES as a covariate is similar to that seen for the other covariates that were looked at here (73.7% for physical activity, 70.5% for alcohol in papers on adults, and 63.6% for hypertension or blood pressure in papers on CVD).
Many of the papers provided information on the relationship between SES and diet. The findings reveal that people with more education typically consume a healthier diet. However, with some aspects of the diet the opposite trend was seen: several papers reported that those with more education consume more sugar-sweetened beverages and more sodium. They also consume more multivitamin supplements. Income also seems to be positively associated with diet quality; however, only a handful of papers provided information on this. That persons with a higher SES consume a generally more nutritious diet is consistent with previous studies (2). Likewise, previous studies have also consistently reported that users of multivitamin supplements are generally better educated25–27.
The findings reported here indicate that failure to include SES in multivariate analysis of cohort studies may be a source of error due to residual confounding. There are several ways by which this might happen. For example, in studies of CVD, error may occur if people of higher SES have better access to the health-care system and are therefore more likely to be screened for risk factors for CVD and to then receive better quality preventive treatment (such as diagnosis and treatment of hypertension). Similarly, error may arise in studies where body weight is the end-point if those of higher SES make greater efforts than those of lower SES to avoid excess weight gain. Conversely, lower SES may increase the risk of disease via psychological pathways, such as by raising the level of stress or by inducing feelings of disempowerment.
In some cohort studies subjects are fairly similar with respect to SES. For example, the Physicians' Health Study included only male physicians28. In such cases failure to adjust for SES is unlikely to lead to a significant error. However, in the large majority of cohort studies the subjects have a fairly wide variation in level of education and income.
Education, income, and social class (or occupation) has each been used as a covariate in the analysis of results from cohort studies. Education has been used far more often than the other two indicators of SES. While all three are closely associated with SES, they are quite distinct. For example, many people have a relatively poor education but still achieve a high income. This often occurs by marriage or by becoming a successful businessperson. Conversely, a person may have a college education but end up with a low income. It follows, therefore, that income (which indicates SES at the present time) may exert more influence on health and behavior than does education (which may have ceased 20 years or more before the cohort study started). Support for this possibility came from an Australian analysis that concluded that income was more strongly associated with diet than was education29. These findings indicate, therefore, that as only a small minority of cohort studies (14.4% of the publications) included income as a covariate, this may be a significant source of error due to residual confounding. It is also possible that the reverse may be true. Education is stable throughout the lifespan and provides individuals with certain abilities related to learning and thinking which are important in matters of health. Measuring income of subjects recruited to a cohort study may often be problematic as many people refuse to divulge their income. For that reason occupation may be a more appropriate measure.
A related question is SES in childhood as this could play a significant role in disease risk in middle age or later. This was suggested by findings from a British cohort study3,4. Adjusting for a range of factors that indicate SES, both those in childhood and adulthood, attenuated the relationship between the plasma concentration of both vitamins C and E and risk of CHD in adults aged over 60. With the exception of that study, I am unaware of any other cohort study that has examined the relationship between SES in childhood and disease risk in adults older than age 50.
As cohort studies vary widely in such features as the country where they are conducted, the age and gender of subjects, the dietary variables being studied, and the health outcomes being investigated, it is likely that the magnitude of the error caused by residual confounding will also be highly variable. More research effort is required to determine the extent to which failure to adjust for SES across the lifecycle (as well as of other covariates) is a source of error in cohort studies.
Another potentially important covariate is growth in the fetal period and early infancy. Fetal development, as indicated by birthweight, as well as the rate of weight gain in early childhood, are predictors of risk of developing heart disease and type 2 diabetes 40 or 50 years later30. While these variables may be only weakly related to SES, they are relevant here as they again underscore how diet, lifestyle, and SES in childhood (or even before birth) may be associated with disease risk decades later.
In conclusion, many cohort studies may have residual confounding caused by the failure to adjust for key covariates. Little attention seems to have been paid to this possible source of significant error. Of particular concern is the question of SES: roughly one third of papers have not included any measure of SES among the covariates. Education was used as the measure of SES far more often than income. However, education is likely to be a weaker measure of present SES than is income. There is a need for more research on the extent to which failure to include income (or another measure of present SES, such as occupation) leads to error in cohort studies.
Papers used as a source of information for this study.
Papers with the same number but different letters (e.g., 2a and 2b) indicate that the same cohort study contributed two separate papers
Views | Downloads | |
---|---|---|
F1000Research | - | - |
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
|
- | - |
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
References
1. Cutler DM, Lleras-Muney A: Understanding differences in health behaviors by education.J Health Econ. 2010; 29 (1): 1-28 PubMed Abstract | Publisher Full TextCompeting Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Invited Reviewers | ||
---|---|---|
1 | 2 | |
Version 2 (revision) 18 May 16 |
read | |
Version 1 07 Sep 15 |
read | read |
Provide sufficient details of any financial or non-financial competing interests to enable users to assess whether your comments might lead a reasonable person to question your impartiality. Consider the following examples, but note that this is not an exhaustive list:
Sign up for content alerts and receive a weekly or monthly email with all newly published articles
Already registered? Sign in
The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.
You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.
You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.
If your email address is registered with us, we will email you instructions to reset your password.
If you think you should have received this email but it has not arrived, please check your spam filters and/or contact for further assistance.
Its nice ... Continue reading Its a good article which has studied the possible importance of income and education as covariates in cohort studies that investigate the relationship between diet and disease.
Its nice to see that the author has mentioned the strengths and weaknesses of the study methodology in the discussion section.
Its nice to see that the author has mentioned the strengths and weaknesses of the study methodology in the discussion section.