ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Research Note

Ask a clearer question, get a better answer.

[version 1; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]
PUBLISHED 25 Sep 2015
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

This article is included in the Innovations and best practices in undergraduate education collection.

Abstract

Many undergraduate students struggle to engage with higher order skills such as evaluation and synthesis in written assignments, either because they do not understand that these are the aim of written assessment or because these critical thinking skills require more effort than writing a descriptive essay. Here, we report that students who attended a freely available workshop, in which they were coached to pose a question in the title of their assignment and then use their essay to answer that question, obtained higher marks for their essay than those who did not attend. We demonstrate that this is not a result of latent academic ability amongst students who chose to attend our workshops and suggest this increase in marks was a result of greater engagement with ‘critical thinking’ skills, which are essential for upper 2:1 and 1st class grades. The tutoring method we used holds two particular advantages: First, we allow students to pick their own topics of interest, which increases ownership of learning, which is associated with motivation and engagement in ‘difficult’ tasks. Second, this method integrates the development of ‘inquisitiveness’ and critical thinking into subject specific learning, which is thought to be more productive than trying to develop these skills in isolation.

Keywords

assessment, essay, critical thinking, critical writing, critical synthesis, inquisitiveness

Introduction

Supporting the development of critical thinking skills in students can be considered to be one of the key goals of most higher education institutions (ten Dam & Volman, 2004). Critical thinking skills such as analysis, evaluation and synthesis represent the highest levels of learning and literacy capabilities, and are highly sought after by employers (CBI, 2009; Krathwohl, 2002; Miller & Tanner, 2015). Despite the focus on teaching critical thinking skills at university, only ~2/3 of UK graduates (lower than the global average) were capable of exhibiting them during a recent literacy skills survey, which is disappointing given the strong correlation between high level skills and employment among graduates (OECD, 2013; OECD, 2015). This paper evaluates a simple method of encouraging students to engage with these higher level skills in their written assessments.

Chanock (2010) outlines five goals that an essay should fulfil. The first is “presenting a question/problem to the reader” and forms the focus of this study. In our experience, students who fail to achieve high grades on written assignments do so because they write descriptive essays lacking a question or problem to solve; i.e they do not understand goal one (Cottrell, 2011). By defending a position or hypothesis using understanding drawn from wider literature, students can provide evidence of high-level literacy and critical thinking (Kellogg & Raulerson, 2007; Miller & Tanner, 2015). Previous studies support the idea that encouraging questioning behaviour promotes the exhibition of critical thinking by students at a range of levels (Commeyras & Summer, 1998; Keeley et al., 1998; Tsui, 2002). In fact, lack of practice performing critical thinking is thought to be a particularly important barrier to the development of higher-level literacy skills (Cottrell, 2011; Kellogg & Raulerson, 2007). We hypothesise that by making the first objective of an essay more obvious to students, and by encouraging them to approach written assignments as questions that need to be answered, students are more likely engage with higher level learning outcomes (Krathwohl, 2002).

A second cited barrier to the development of higher-level literacy skills is reticence on the part of the student, as essays containing evaluation and synthesis are more difficult to write than descriptive essays (Cottrell, 2011; Keeley et al., 1995). An important aspect of our method involves allowing the students to choose what question they are most interested in within the defined subject area. In this study each student was asked to explore a broader concept associated with a specific animal behaviour they chose to study earlier in the year (for details see Methods section). We believe that the sense of ownership of the task could help to improve engagement with ‘difficult’, higher-learning outcomes, as motivation to engage with studying is thought to be positively associated with personal interest in the topic (Pintrich, 2003).

We propose that coaching students to pose a question in their essay title can integrate ‘inquisitiveness’ development into written assessments. Integration of the development of these skills into the context of the course has been argued as being more effective than trying to teach these skills in a separate course (ten Dam & Volman, 2004; Wingate, 2006). We suggest that students who start by posing a question in the title are more likely to understand the first of Chanock’s (2010) aims of an essay as well as being more likely to exhibit higher-level literacy skills throughout their writing. Thus, we hypothesise that students who pose a question in their title will obtain higher assessment scores than those who do not, as evidence of higher-level skills are essential in obtaining higher marks.

Methods

Participants

Project participants were students enrolled on a second year undergraduate module Behavioural Ecology (UK level 5, 20 credits). We believe this group of 55 individuals to be typical of the wider population of UK undergraduate students enrolled on Honours Degree Programs in the Biological Sciences. 23 of the students were male and 32 were female.

Assessment of the module was by an end of module written examination (50%) and summative coursework (50%). This coursework comprised three tasks (A, B and C) worth 10%, 10% and 30% respectively. Task A required pairs of students to work together to find a short (3 minute) video clip of animals performing a behaviour that interested them and to complete a written assessment in the form of briefing notes for a film crew interested in recreating the video as part of a wildlife documentary. This task encourages observation and description. Task B required individual students to self-assess task A and reflect (in writing) upon their use of the assessment criteria in doing so. This task encourages students to think about the assessment criteria and the way in which they are applied. Task C required students to write a detailed essay exploring the underlying principles and wider context of the behaviour chosen for assessment A. The notes provided to students to explain these tasks are available as supplementary material (Appendix 1).

This project investigates the impact of an optional workshop-based intervention that took place after the students had received grades and feedback on assessments A & B and before they completed assessment C. All students were invited to attend a workshop led by DH and LM as preparation for assessment C with a focus on improving essay writing skills. All students were provided equal opportunity to attend; multiple timeslots were available for students with other commitments. At the workshop, DH & LM explained the function of a good essay in that it should outline a problem that needs to be solved, then present and evaluate the various solutions using wider literature. We suggested that in order to help the students do this they should present a question that needs answering as the essay title, and then use the essay to answer that question with reference to the broader literature (see Appendix 2 for essay titles). We then helped students create a relevant question to ask, suggesting they avoid descriptive ‘how’ questions, and focused on evaluative ‘why’ or ‘to what degree’ type questions.

This activity was not conceived as a research project and because attendance at the workshop was optional student attendance was not monitored. For the purpose of this study we assumed that students who posed a question in the title of their essay had attended the workshop and understood the underlying concepts of the workshop, and this has been used as the independent factor in our analysis. We acknowledge that this lack of certainty in the allocation of students to the did/did not attend category does need to be borne in mind when interpreting our results. Another possible confounding factor is that voluntary workshop attendance may be skewed towards individuals who are more engaged or motivated with the module; and these individuals are more likely to obtain higher grades because of this higher engagement with the module content (Pintrich, 2003). We have controlled for inherent capability or engagement of the student in this study by including the previous mark on Assessment A of the student as an independent factor in our statistical analyses (see Statistics). Students’ essays were marked by an assessor who was not involved in the delivery of the module or aware of the purpose of the workshops but who does have the relevant disciplinary expertise (GS), so as to not influence student grades.

Ethics

Ethical approval for publication of our study was obtained from the University of Hull, SoBBEs ethics board (Code H038). As the significance of the results presented here was only noted after marking had taken place, it was not possible to obtain student approval. However, students cannot be identified individually from the study results or data set, which was deemed sufficient by the ethics board.

Statistics

A generalised linear mixed effect model (GLMM) was used to test for an effect of posing a question as the essay title on the percentage mark awarded to the essay. The student’s mark on assignment A was included as a second independent variable to control for the effect of inherent capability. As assignment A was written in pairs, the pair groups were included as a random factor (random intercepts) to control for non-independence of the marks. An observation level, random factor was included to account for overdispersion (Harrison, 2014). As the dependent factor was a percentage, the GLMM was run with a Binomial error structure. All statistics were performed in R using the glmer function in the lme4 package (Bates, 2010) of R v3.1 (R Development Core Team, 2014). The Minimum Adequate Model was established via log-likelihood ratio comparisons using Maximum Likelihood approximation, for which X2 results indicating significance are reported (Bates, 2010).

Results

Essays with a question in the title scored significantly higher than those without (X21= 4.62, P= 0.03; Figure 1 & Table 1). There was no significant effect of score of previous assignment (X21= 3.02, P= 0.10), or interaction between the two independent variables (question in title:previous score, X21= 0.81, P= 0.36).

b0a4cc01-e368-443c-8524-cd67649aa154_figure1.gif

Figure 1. Box plots comparing % scores across essays that included a question in the title and those that did not.

Table 1. Mean percentage scores and standard deviations of students who did and did not pose a question in their essay title.

Did the student ask a
question in the title?
Mean score
(%)
Standard
deviation
(+/-%)
Titles with a question7010
Titles without a question6310

Discussion & Conclusion

Our results support our original hypothesis that students who posed a question in the title of their essay would obtain higher grades than those who did not. We suggest that this is because the process of coaching students to use questions to think ‘inquisitively’ improves the likelihood they will engage with critical thinking skills, such as analysis, evaluation and synthesis. Our results support this because evidence of these skills is necessary for work to be awarded 1st class grades (70% or higher), and we note the much higher proportion of students posing a question who obtained a 1st class grade (44% [asked a question] vs. 22% [did not ask a question]). Given the importance of critical thinking skills for obtaining higher degree classifications, better literacy scores and gaining employment following graduation, we suggest this outlook may be added to the methods of developing student essay skills (CBI, 2009; OECD, 2013; OECD, 2015). Our method is particularly advantageous because it can be easily integrated into the curricula; as opposed to needing to be taught separately (ten Dam & Volman, 2004; Wingate, 2006). Furthermore, the workshop method in our study focused on helping students develop their own questions to answer, encouraging student ownership and motivation in order to overcome any reticence to engage in ‘difficult’ higher-level literacy skills (Cottrell, 2011; Keeley et al., 1995).

It is important to state that we believe the whole process of teaching students to think in terms of questions/problems and how to answer them is important; as opposed to merely the act of placing a question in the title. We also do not suggest that this is a blanket method of encouraging students to develop high-level literacy/critical thinking skills; evidently, some of our students who attended the workshop and used the method did poorly (hence did not grasp the underlying concept) and other students did well despite not using the method detailed herein (Figure 1). This is to be expected where students construct their knowledge base and its application individually and thus respond differently to instruction based on their prior experiences and learning preferences, and does not undermine its validity as a potential tool for broader teaching strategies (ten Dam & Volman, 2004). We concede that a more extensive study, including more students across multiple assessments, is required to resolutely confirm the trends found herein. Further work should focus on helping students to distinguish between descriptive ‘how’ questions and evaluative ‘why’ questions to see if this further improve the efficacy of the method.

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 25 Sep 2015
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Henri D, Morrell L and Scott G. Ask a clearer question, get a better answer. [version 1; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2015, 4:901 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.7066.1)
NOTE: If applicable, it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 25 Sep 2015
Views
34
Cite
Reviewer Report 13 Oct 2015
Kay Yeoman, School of Biological Sciences, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 34
Critical thinking is certainly a key skill which we would like our students to develop during their time in Higher Education. The paper points to the OECD studies showing that students tend to lack these critical thinking skills. I think ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Yeoman K. Reviewer Report For: Ask a clearer question, get a better answer. [version 1; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2015, 4:901 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.7606.r10553)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Views
23
Cite
Reviewer Report 07 Oct 2015
Rachel Stubbington, School of Science and Technology, Nottingham Trent University, Nottingham, UK 
Approved
VIEWS 23
This paper considers a workshop conducted to support students preparing a summative written assignment, which had the unplanned benefit of improving critical thinking skills, as evidenced by higher assignment grades. The development of higher-order thinking skills in undergraduate students is ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Stubbington R. Reviewer Report For: Ask a clearer question, get a better answer. [version 1; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2015, 4:901 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.7606.r10704)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 28 Oct 2015
    Dominic Henri, School of Biological, Biomedical and Environmental Sciences, University of Hull, Hull, HU6 7RX, UK
    28 Oct 2015
    Author Response
    Thank you very much, some really helpful points here. We will create a full response when we hear back from the third referee.
    Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 28 Oct 2015
    Dominic Henri, School of Biological, Biomedical and Environmental Sciences, University of Hull, Hull, HU6 7RX, UK
    28 Oct 2015
    Author Response
    Thank you very much, some really helpful points here. We will create a full response when we hear back from the third referee.
    Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 25 Sep 2015
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.