ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Review
Revised

Blinding in trials of interventional procedures is possible and worthwhile

[version 2; peer review: 3 approved]
PUBLISHED 30 Jan 2018
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

Abstract

In this paper, we use evidence from our earlier review of surgical randomised controlled trials with a placebo arm to show that blinding in trials of interventional procedures is feasible. We give examples of ingenious strategies that have been used to simulate the active procedure and to make the placebo control indistinguishable from the active treatment. We discuss why it is important to blind of patients, assessors, and caregivers and what types of bias that may occur in interventional trials. Finally, we describe the benefits of blinding, from the obvious ones such as avoiding bias, as well as less evident benefits such as avoiding patient drop out in the control arm.

Keywords

blinding, RCT, randomised controlled trials, placebo, surgery, procedures, non-pharmacological

Revised Amendments from Version 1

The differences between these two versions consist of added definitions and references - as requested by the Reviewers, and some minor edits of spelling, grammar, and punctuation that were missed on the original submission.

See the authors' detailed response to the review by Helene Moustgaard and Asbjørn Hróbjartsson
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Harald Walach
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Ian A Harris

Introduction

The aim of a trial is to produce unbiased evidence. As randomised controlled trials (RCTs) with a placebo arm control for many types of bias and have high internal validity they are regarded as a reliable method of demonstrating treatment efficacy1,2. RCTs of interventional procedures are rare36, partly because they are challenging7; however, they are not impossible to perform, even if they involve a placebo arm8. In this paper we discuss why trials should be blinded and summarise methods that have been used to achieve blinding in the published placebo-controlled trials of interventional procedures.

Blinding in interventional trials is often necessary because, nowadays, many procedures are performed to reduce pain and improve function, and quality of life. Pain, function and quality of life are sometimes regarded as preferable outcome measures because they reflect patients’ needs and point of view9. However, as these outcomes depend on patients’ subjective perception, they are prone to bias and may lead to an exaggerated treatment effect in open-label trials1013. Using subjective outcomes in an open-label study undermines its internal validity because it makes it impossible to determine how much of the reported effect is related to the investigated treatment and how much is related to various forms of bias.

It is important to note that controlling for bias comes at a price. Because of the standardised conditions under which they are performed, the uncertainty of treatment allocation, and the presence of assessors, RCTs differ from everyday patient care; therefore, they often have low external validity14. Moreover, the size of the effect in blinded trials tends to be smaller than in clinical practice because of the inherent uncertainty of the treatment allocation in a trial15.

Definitions

In this paper, we have defined surgery like in our previous publication8 as “any interventional procedure that changes the anatomy and requires a skin incision or the use of endoscopic techniques; dental studies were excluded. We used the term placebo to refer to a surgical placebo, a sham surgery, or an imitation procedure intended to mimic the active intervention; including the scenario when a scope was inserted and nothing was done but patients were sedated or under general anaesthesia and could not distinguish whether or not they underwent the actual surgery”8. Also the type of outcomes was defined in the same way as in our earlier paper16 i.e., “Outcomes were classified as “subjective”, i.e., patient-reported and depending on the patients’ perception and cooperation, “assessed”, i.e., subjective ratings by external assessors and depending upon their judgment, and “objective”, i.e., measured using devices or laboratory tests and independent of patients’ or observers’ perception, for example, weight.”

Blinding of patients, surgeons, outcome assessors, and care-givers

Blinding of patients

Blinding means concealing the treatment allocation from patients and any other people involved in the trial who may bias the results of the trial by knowing which groups the patients were randomised to.

Blinding of patients prevents reporting bias in patient-reported measures. For example, it has been demonstrated that non-blinded patients may exaggerate the effects size by 0.56 standard deviations and that the effect is even larger in studies on interventional procedures, such as acupuncture17,18. This bias may be caused by patients’ expectations of treatment effect and information given to them before the treatment19. Patients may also report symptoms depending on their “hunches” about treatment being effective or they may give answers they believe are “correct” or expected from them, for example, because it would have been impolite not to report improvement17. Therefore, it has been suggested that patients should be blinded whenever possible17.

Blinding of patients also reduces adherence bias, i.e. patients in the control group not following the protocol. It may also prevent so called “contamination of the control group”, i.e. seeking additional treatment outside the trial and receiving concomitant treatment. Blinding also improves patient retention in the trial. Risk of attrition in blinded trials is about 4% whereas in non-blinded trials it is 7%17. Specifically, in placebo-controlled surgical trials, subject retention is often reported as “excellent”20, and in our analysis the withdrawal rate was low (4%) and comparable between the treatment and the placebo arm21.

Blinding of surgeons

Unlike drug trials, in which the physician gives a tablet prepared somewhere else, the surgeon has to perform a specific procedure considered to be therapeutic; therefore, blinding of surgeons may not always be possible.

There have been attempts to blind surgeons, for example, a surgeon inserted a catheter under fluoroscopic guidance and handed over the procedure to a technician who delivered the radiofrequency energy (or not) according to the allocation22. In other trials, a palatal implant delivery system was prepared by the manufacturer to either contain the implant or not, which allowed for blinding of surgeons23,24.

Blinding of outcome assessors

In 81% of placebo-controlled surgical trials both patients and assessors were blinded21. It has been demonstrated that, non-blinded assessors of subjective outcomes cause less bias in trials than non-blinded patients reporting their symptoms25. Blinding of assessors prevents observer-related bias, detection bias, and the Pygmalion effect. The Pygmalion effect, also called the Rosenthal effect, refers to a situation when investigators looking for a particular response are predisposed to interpret the result in a way that shows the response they expect, even if it is objectively absent26. Interestingly, a study by Hróbjartsson and colleagues demonstrated that non-blinded assessors were over-optimistic and “over-rated” patients in the treatment group rather than “under-rated” patients in the control group27. To minimise the assessor-related bias, in some trials the assessment was done by people not involved in the surgery, for example, by staff at another hospital28, or by a pathologist blinded to the treatment allocation29.

Blinding of care-givers

Apart from blinding patients and assessors, it is important that care-givers and clinical or research staff also do not know patient treatment allocation, because their behaviour and attitudes may influence patient responses3032. Patient-clinician interaction plays an important role in treatment response, and patients in trials do better as they get more attention and time from clinical staff than patients receiving standard care33,34. Therefore, the interactions between patients and the trial team should be standardised so that the “treatment context” (similar attention from doctors, expectations, and settings) is comparable between the groups.

Strategies used to maintain blinding in interventional placebo-controlled trials

A placebo-controlled RCT is a special type of a trial, in which one of the control arms involves an imitation procedure that seems identical but does not involve the crucial element believed to be "the cure". A placebo arm is necessary to demonstrate that the observed improvement is really caused by the investigated procedure as it controls for the effects of receiving treatment other than the crucial surgical element.

It is often difficult to determine what is a specific and what is a non-specific effect in a trial34,35, and to disentangle a placebo response from response bias or the effect of patient-doctor interactions36. It is beyond the scope of this review to discuss definitions of placebo1,36. Whether something is or is not a placebo depends on the intervention and chosen outcome variables1, but in order for blinding to be successful, the control procedure has to be as similar as possible to the investigated procedure34. Interventional trials differ from drug trials as they require access to the anatomical structure of interest; therefore, they involve a skin incision or an insertion of a scope.

In many published trials, blinding during the surgery was straightforward because patients were under general anaesthesia or heavy sedation and, therefore; unaware of the details of surgical procedures. In such trials, only the surgical wound had to be similar in both groups. Some studies did not add any placebo procedure but simply omitted part of interventional procedure, for example, in the trial by Stone and colleagues, all patients underwent a percutaneous coronary intervention and maximal medical therapy but only patients in the active arm also had percutaneous transmyocardial revascularisation37.

When light sedation or local anaesthesia were used, surgical staff had to simulate the actual intervention to preserve the blinding. The complexity of a surgical procedure made blinding challenging, and ingenious ideas were required to make the real and placebo interventions indistinguishable.

Imitation of incision/ surgical access point

If a procedure requires open surgery, then it leaves an obvious mark where the incision has been made, which has to be imitated in the placebo group. There have been very few trials involving full-skin incision, in both the surgical and placebo arms. In the seminal trials on internal mammary artery ligation38,39 a skin incision was made to expose the arteries in all patients, but no ligation was made in patients in the placebo group. Similarly, Guyuron and colleagues used a skin incision to expose superficial nerves and muscles, which were cut during the active surgery, but in the placebo group, the integrity of these structures was maintained40. Trials investigating transplantation of dopaminergic neurones as a treatment for Parkinson’s disease not only required skin incision but also burr holes in the skull, but in the placebo group the burr holes did not penetrate the dura matter41,42.

Most of the published placebo-controlled surgical trials used minimally-invasive methods to access the structure of interest. For example, the placebo procedure involved laparoscopy but without ablation43, endoscopy without radiofrequency energy delivery44, bronchoscopy without radiofrequency energy delivery45, or bronchoscopy without a valve placement46. Therefore, most of the studies required a small incision to mimic the portals created during the laparoscopy or arthroscopy, or to mimic the incision through which an intravascular catheter was inserted47. Interestingly, Sutton and colleagues used three incisions in both groups, so that patients could not tell apart a diagnostic laparoscopy from a laparoscopic surgery; even though the third instrument port was not necessary in the placebo group48. Trials using endoscopy and bronchoscopy were even easier to blind as natural orifices were used to insert the scope, and the incision or actual procedure site was not visible to patients, care-givers, and assessors.

Simulation of an interventional procedure

Typically, the preparation for the placebo and the active procedure was as similar as possible and imitated the visual, auditory, and physical cues4952. In order to mimic the sounds, surgeons were required to talk through the procedure steps53, ask for instruments54,55, use suction55 or ask for a laser or other device to be activated, even though it was not used in the placebo group5660.

Clinical staff performing the intervention were screened from the patients’ view61, either by a surgical drape59 or by arranging the operating room in a way that the patient could not see the procedure51. In a trial by Stone and colleagues, patients were heavily sedated and wore opaque goggles37. In a trial by Maurer and colleagues, the manufacturer delivered tools that looked identical but those for the placebo group did not contain an implant, which allowed for blinding of patients and clinical staff24.

Surgeons also attempted to imitate sensory cues, for example, by manipulating the knee as if the actual arthroscopy were performed55, injecting saline to imitate tidal irrigation20, or by splashing saline on the knee to simulate lavage52. In a trial on meniscectomy, Sihvonen and colleagues used a mechanised shaver (without the blade) pushing it firmly against the patella to simulate the sensations the patient would experience during the surgery55. In a trial on intragastric balloon for obesity, operators manipulated the endoscope as during the balloon insertion to create the sensation of resistance in the stomach60.

Even smell during the surgery was imitated to make the placebo procedure indistinguishable from surgery. For example, in the trial by Deviere and colleagues there were concerns that patients could have known the allocation because the copolymer used in the active arm had a distinct smell62. In trials on vertebroplasty, a container with cement was opened during placebo procedure to help with blinding by imitating the smell49,63.

Inactive nature of the placebo

It is important that the procedure used for blinding does not have any therapeutic effect. For example, the results of the vertebroplasty trials43,63 were criticised because the elements of placebo procedure could have had an effect on the reported pain, namely, a potential pharmacological anaesthesia due to injection of an anaesthetic into the facet capsule and periosteum64.

On the other hand, the procedure used for blinding may have diagnostic use, as with diagnostic laparoscopy43,48,65 or diagnostic laparoscopy with biopsy66. In the trial by Sihvonen and colleagues, all participants underwent diagnostic arthroscopy, but only after they had been confirmed to be eligible for inclusion in the trial was the envelope with the assignment opened and the assignment revealed to the surgeon55.

Duration of the procedure

Many trials specifically stated that the duration of procedure in the surgical and control arms were matched, either by imitating the elements of the surgical procedure or by keeping all patients in the operation room for the same duration of time41,45,52,54,55,67,68. However, in some trials, the placebo procedure was shortened in comparison to the actual surgery because it was believed it would have been ethically unacceptable to prolong the placebo intervention56,62.

Additional procedures that may reveal allocation

Interventional treatment often requires additional procedures, such as diagnostic scans or medication to prevent infection50,63,69, blood clots47, transplant rejection70, or epileptic fits42. For example, in the trial by Freed and colleagues, both groups received identical preoperative evaluation, intraoperative sedation and pain control, underwent two PET scans and a MRI scan, and received phenytoin42. In some trials, the same medication was given in both groups, whereas in others unnecessary treatment was omitted or imitated, for example, by injecting saline instead of antibiotics71.

Standardisation of interventions and care

The active and placebo procedure have to be indistinguishable but they also have to be stable and standardised. Standardisation of the procedure itself may be difficult but is important because surgeons vary in their experience and gain experience throughout the trial.

Some of the changes observed in a trial may not be related to the treatment or the placebo intervention, but may be caused by the natural course of the disease, spontaneous remissions or fluctuations in the severity of symptoms or regression to the mean34,72. Some changes may be a result of just being in a trial either because of lifestyle changes that are part of the protocol such as self-monitoring, using diaries, or avoiding alcohol, or due to so called “Hawthorne effect”, which refers to change in the behaviour when people, both patients and doctors, know that they are being observed34. Finally, it has been demonstrated that adhering to a protocol improves the performance of doctors, and that patients who adhere to treatment regimens have better outcomes73. Therefore, it is important to standardise pre- and post-operative care, and the explanations given to the patients. For example, in a trial by Sihvonen and colleagues, all procedures were standardised and recorded on video; the post-operative care was also standardised, and all patients received the same exercise program and walking aids55.

Blinding after the surgery

Most trials blinded the assessors while the surgeon and other staff in the operating room were aware of the group assignment, and did not participate in further treatment, post-operative care or follow-up of the patient43,55,74. In a trial by Thomsen and colleagues, the post-operative care and assessment was done at a different hospital than the surgery28. In a trial by Cotton and colleagues, the post-operative care was provided by the referring physician, who was blinded when deciding on treatment, and when this was not sufficient, by the evaluating physician at the study site (who was also blinded)74.

Bias specific to surgical trials

There are other types of bias that are specific to surgical trials. They are mostly related to patients not being entered into the trial because their symptoms are too severe or not severe enough to justify surgery or because the anatomical conditions or technical difficulties make the surgery impossible to perform. For example, in trials on upper gastrointestinal tract bleeding, the endoscopic procedure was not performed if the rate of blood loss was too fast, or the endoscopy was judged to be life-threatening and posed an unacceptable risk7577. This meant that only patients with less severe symptoms were included in the study. In other trials, some patients were excluded because they could not tolerate endoscopy, or due to anatomical conditions that made the surgery impossible to carry out, for example, not being able to aim a laser at the bleeding arteries77. Alternatively, some patients were not included in a trial because they were no longer eligible, for example because the bleeding had stopped77 or they no longer reported the symptoms on the day of the study78. These confounders are difficult to predict and control for.

Conclusions

Blinding in trials of interventional procedures is possible and many creative methods have been used to maintain the blinding. Interventional procedures are challenging to blind, but the effort is worthwhile because of the obvious benefits, such as avoiding bias, as well as the less evident benefits, such as avoiding patient drop-out in the control arm.

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 2
VERSION 2 PUBLISHED 08 Sep 2017
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Wartolowska K, Beard D and Carr A. Blinding in trials of interventional procedures is possible and worthwhile [version 2; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2018, 6:1663 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.12528.2)
NOTE: If applicable, it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 08 Sep 2017
Views
16
Cite
Reviewer Report 04 Dec 2017
Helene Moustgaard, Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine, Odense University Hospital (OUH), University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark 
Asbjørn Hróbjartsson, Centre for Evidence-Based Medicine, Odense University Hospital (OUH), University of Southern Denmark, Odense, Denmark 
Approved
VIEWS 16
The overall topic of this paper is important: Blinding facilitates unbiased trial results, but blinding is less used in surgical trials (and less frequently discussed) than in trials of pharmacological interventions. A prevalent preconception is that blinding is not possible, ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Moustgaard H and Hróbjartsson A. Reviewer Report For: Blinding in trials of interventional procedures is possible and worthwhile [version 2; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2018, 6:1663 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.13566.r25901)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 30 Jan 2018
    Karolina Wartolowska, University of Oxford, UK
    30 Jan 2018
    Author Response
    Our aim was to discuss the methodological role of blinding in surgical trials and to demonstrate that blinding can and should be done in surgical trials, especially, if the outcomes ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 30 Jan 2018
    Karolina Wartolowska, University of Oxford, UK
    30 Jan 2018
    Author Response
    Our aim was to discuss the methodological role of blinding in surgical trials and to demonstrate that blinding can and should be done in surgical trials, especially, if the outcomes ... Continue reading
Views
9
Cite
Reviewer Report 04 Dec 2017
Harald Walach, Department of Pediatric Gastroenterology, Medical University Poznan, Poznań, Poland 
Approved
VIEWS 9
This review is, as its self-set goal goes, very well substantiated, balanced and timely. There is no factual correction in itself that is necessary in my view.
 
However, I wish to give a few critical feedbacks to ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Walach H. Reviewer Report For: Blinding in trials of interventional procedures is possible and worthwhile [version 2; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2018, 6:1663 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.13566.r28006)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 30 Jan 2018
    Karolina Wartolowska, University of Oxford, UK
    30 Jan 2018
    Author Response
    We are grateful for the comment regarding the hierarchy of evidence. We have rephrased the sentence so that it now reads “high level of evidence because they attempt to minimise ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 30 Jan 2018
    Karolina Wartolowska, University of Oxford, UK
    30 Jan 2018
    Author Response
    We are grateful for the comment regarding the hierarchy of evidence. We have rephrased the sentence so that it now reads “high level of evidence because they attempt to minimise ... Continue reading
Views
13
Cite
Reviewer Report 20 Sep 2017
Ian A Harris, Whitlam Orthopaedic Research Centre, Ingham Institute for Applied Medical Research, South Western Sydney Clinical School, University of New South Wales,  Liverpool, NSW, Australia 
Approved
VIEWS 13
This manuscript is a narrative overview of methods of blinding in trials of procedures, using the previous review by these authors as the source of data. It explains the rationale for blinding and gives examples (subgrouped) of how blinding was ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Harris IA. Reviewer Report For: Blinding in trials of interventional procedures is possible and worthwhile [version 2; peer review: 3 approved]. F1000Research 2018, 6:1663 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.13566.r25898)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 30 Jan 2018
    Karolina Wartolowska, University of Oxford, UK
    30 Jan 2018
    Author Response
    We are grateful for the comment and we have added an explanation that trials have limited external validity, as by controlling for confounding factors and standardising the trial conditions, they ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 30 Jan 2018
    Karolina Wartolowska, University of Oxford, UK
    30 Jan 2018
    Author Response
    We are grateful for the comment and we have added an explanation that trials have limited external validity, as by controlling for confounding factors and standardising the trial conditions, they ... Continue reading

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 2
VERSION 2 PUBLISHED 08 Sep 2017
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.