ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Research Article

Toward a paradigm shift from deficit-based to proactive speech and language treatment: Randomized pilot trial of the Babble Boot Camp in infants with classic galactosemia

[version 1; peer review: 2 approved with reservations]
PUBLISHED 11 Mar 2019
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

Abstract

Background: Speech or language therapy is typically initiated reactively after a child starts showing delays. Infants with classic galactosemia (CG), an inborn error of metabolism with a known high risk for both speech and language disorders, hold the keys towards evaluating whether preventive treatment is effective when the risks are known at birth.  We present pilot data from a randomized parallel trial of an innovative proactive speech and language intervention program, the Babble Boot Camp (BBC). 
Method: Five children with CG, otherwise healthy, participated in the BBC from approximately 2 to 24 months of age. One of these was randomly selected as control receiving conventional management. A pediatric speech-language pathologist met weekly via telepractice with the parents in the treatment cohort. Parents implemented the prespeech, speech, and language stimulation and expansion activities according to the protocol. The control child was still too young for conventional treatment. Primary outcome measures were speech sound production complexity in babble and speech and expressive vocabulary size. Secondary outcome measures were developmental milestones in communication, motor, and cognition. Outcomes in the treatment cohort were compared to typical children and the control child. The trial is ongoing.
Results:  All four treated children had higher speech sound skills in babble, three had higher speech sound skills in meaningful speech, two had higher expressive vocabularies, and three had higher communication and personal-social skills, compared to the control child with CG.
Discussion: Given the high risk for speech and language delays in children with CG, finding on-schedule abilities in two or more of the treated children but not the untreated child is unexpected under random conditions. The trends toward beneficial effects of the BBC on speech sound production, expressive language, and communication milestones warrant appropriately powered larger clinical trials with full randomization.
Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov NCT03838016 (12th February 2019).

Keywords

speech disorder, language impairment, genetic risk, infant, very early intervention, prevention

Introduction

Difficulties with speech and language are common among young children. In the US, 11% of children age 3 to 6 years have a communication disorder (Black et al., 2015). Many parents who are concerned about their child’s ability to talk ask the child’s doctor, who, in turn, may refer them to a speech-language pathologist. By the time a referral is made, the child may already be two, three, or even four years old and has passed critical stages in the process of speech and language development (Cates et al., 2012). There is strong evidence that early interventions in children with known risks or first signs of a variety of disorders are highly effective, for instance interventions for children with autism spectrum disorder as young as 12 months (Dawson et al., 2010; Guralnick, 2011; Rogers et al., 2014). However, very early speech and language services are not yet available, in part because speech and language are later-developing skills and disorders in these areas cannot be reliably diagnosed on behavioral grounds until an age when deficits become evident, greater than 24 months for speech (Goldman & Fristoe, 2015) and greater than 36 months for language (Semel et al., 2004).

There are some early red flags during infancy that may foreshadow a later diagnosis of speech sound disorder and language impairment. Examples are sparse and delayed babbling (American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 2007; DePaolis et al., 2013; McCune & Vihman, 2001; Oller et al., 1998; Oller et al., 1999; Stoel-Gammon, 1989; Stoel-Gammon, 2011; Vihman & Greenlee, 1987; Vihman et al., 1985), low engagement in activities with joint attention (Mundy et al., 2007), and failure to use gestures (Rowe & Goldin-Meadow, 2009). Specifically regarding babbling, a close predictive link between quantity/quality of babble and speech and language development later on has been previously described in typical children (American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 2007; DePaolis et al., 2013; McCune & Vihman, 2001; Oller et al., 1998; Oller et al., 1999; Stoel-Gammon, 1989; Stoel-Gammon, 2011; Vihman & Greenlee, 1987; Vihman et al., 1985). However, whether treatment targeting these early signs of communication has a beneficial effect on later speech and language development is unknown because such treatments have not been developed and validated. Going even further back in the developmental trajectory, some children are born with risk factors for speech and language disorders and this risk is known long before prespeech behaviors such as coo and babble and actual speech and language emerge. The question is whether proactive, preventive treatment, if it existed, could reduce the deleterious effects associated with the disease in these cases and thereby improve outcomes.

Infants with classic galactosemia (CG) are an ideal population to investigate whether proactive interventions during the first two years of life, long before traditional assessment and intervention are available, can significantly improve speech and language outcomes. CG is a recessively inherited inborn error of metabolism diagnosed via newborn screening, with incidence rates in the US ranging from 1/30,000 to 1/60,000. Newborn diagnosis can be life saving because of the deleterious effects of galactose buildup in the child’s blood that can occur if dietary restrictions are not implemented immediately. Despite rigorous dietary management, however, children with this disease have a substantially higher risk, compared to the typically developing (TD) population, not only for motor and learning disabilities (Antshel et al., 2004; Karadag et al., 2013; Potter et al., 2013) but also, importantly, for severe speech and language disorders. Speech disorders were reported in 77% of children with CG (Hughes et al., 2009), compared to 3.8% among children generally (Shriberg et al., 1999), and language impairment in 90% of children with CG (Waggoner et al., 1990), compared to 7.4% among children generally (Tomblin et al., 1997). This elevated risk, coupled with the early identification, makes the CG population ideal to examine the efficacy of prospective intervention therapy. If proactive intervention is shown to be more effective than conventional management, this has the potential to change the management model from deficit-based to preventive services for these infants. It will also motivate similar studies in infants with other types of risk for communication disorders, for instance very low birth weight and 7q11.23 duplication syndrome. That is, children known to be at risk may benefit from early, prospective intervention, thus improving outcomes.

The Babble Boot Camp (BBC) is a program of activities and routines designed for children during the pre-speech and very early speech and language stages. It contains components intended to shape dyadic interactions across modalities, stimulate earliest vocalizations (coo, babble), support emergence of first words and sentences, and foster vocabulary and syntax growth. The active phase of intervention covers ages 2 to 24 months, with plans for follow-up testing using a professional evaluation of speech, language, and cognitive abilities at ages 36 and 48 months.

Here, we report pilot results of the BBC. This Phase 0 exploratory study demonstrates, with clear clinical application, a viable proactive early intervention approach for minimizing speech and language disorders in a vulnerable population of infants with a known genetic risk for these disorders. The purpose of this pilot study was to examine if children with CG participating in the BBC show age-expected markers of expressive speech and language and to compare their outcomes to those in the control child. The primary focus is speech and language development measured with standardized assessments, with secondary attention to cognitive and motor development.

Methods

This study was conducted with approval of the Institutional Review Boards at Arizona State University (IRB ID # STUDY00004969) and Washington State University (IRB ID # 13099). It began on January 31, 2017, and the trial is ongoing. Parents learned about the study through online research announcements and referrals from physicians or other service providers and contacted the research team. Once eligibility for participation was established and parents made the decision to participate, they gave written permission for their infants’ participation and written consent for their own participation. The study is listed on ClinicalTrials.gov under NCT03838016.

Participants

The current participants are 14 children with CG and their parents. Here, we report on a subset of the children, the five oldest children with the longest participation record, for whom a nearly complete dataset is available through age 21 months. This pilot treatment cohort consists of four children, two girls (codes CG1, CG2) and two boys (CG4, CG5). Note that CG1 only participated in the study up to age 18 months, due to personal circumstances. One additional boy with CG was randomly selected to serve as a control who did not participate in the BBC treatment program. All families participated in the close monitoring components of the study, described further below. In the near future, this parallel design will be built out by recruiting more children into the treatment and control cohorts using randomization and blinding, the latter of which can only be applied to research team members who analyze the data.

In- and exclusionary criteria are identical for the treatment and control children with CG and are designed to evaluate the effects of the treatment while keeping all other factors the same. Age at entry into the study is approximately 2 months. All infants are required to have a newborn diagnosis of CG. Infants with additional health or sensory diagnoses are excluded. Boys and girls of any racial/ethnic background are equally eligible to participate. Primary language in the home must be English and at least one parent must have at least an 8th grade education. Because the intervention is implemented via telepractice software, any family whose primary language is English can participate, regardless of country of residence. The cohort as a whole currently includes families in the US and the UK.

Materials and procedures

The BBC is implemented via parent training by a speech-language pathologist (SLP) with expertise in early childhood. The SLP uses a HIPAA-compliant telepractice computer interface to connect with the families. Parents learn about the typical milestones of prespeech, speech, and language development, potential red flags for delays, and, importantly, specific activities that support typical development for all stages of the program and development beyond.

Following an orientation to the program, the SLP meets with each family once per week for approximately 10 minutes to train and consult on the relevant activities given the child’s current speech and language status. Examples of activities are stimulating and reinforcing coos and babble, enriching the child’s linguistic environment with joint book reading (Storkel et al., 2017), pointing out the names of objects (Dimitrova et al., 2016), and expanding child utterances to provide slightly more complex model sentences (Hassink & Leonard, 2010). Activities and routines are modeled on typical developmental milestones for ages 2 to 24 months reported in the scientific literature (Chapman, 2000; Gordon-Brannan & Weiss, 2007; MacLeod, 2013; Paul & Norbury, 2012, Stoel-Gammon, 1983; Stoel-Gammon, 2011). Children in the BBC treatment cohort progress through these milestones not based on age but on present levels of ability.

One key principle underlying all activities is the zone of proximal development or scaffolding in which parents provide speech and language models that bridge what the child can already do and what is slightly beyond the child’s skill set: the model is in the zone of skills that the child can do with help (Vygotsky, 1979). One key skill that is targeted throughout the program is imitation.

Outcome measures

Because the BBC is designed to beneficially influence speech and language development, the primary outcome variables are speech sound production in babble and speech and expressive language ability. Secondary areas of interests are cognitive and motor development and quality of life, but these are only partially addressed in this report. During the active BBC phase, we monitor all of these areas as well as a range of other enrichment variables including volubility of child vocalizations, environmental influences, and demographic factors.

Specifically to assess speech sound development and language growth, we use a combination of several standardized tests and established clinical procedures. In the present pilot report, we quantify speech and language growth using several metrics. First, we focus on the complexity of the produced speech sounds during babble and early speech. Once per month per child starting at age 6 months, we compute the Mean Babbling Level (MBL) (Stoel-Gammon, 1989), a clinical measure of speech sound complexity for babbling. To compute the MBL, a set of at least 50 utterances is compiled and transcribed into the International Phonetic Alphabet. An expert rater assigns a score of 1, 2, or 3 to each utterance and computes the average; thus, MBL scores range from 1 to 3 for each child at any given time point. A score of 1 is assigned to simple utterances consisting of a vowel, a syllabic consonant, or a consonant-vowel (CV) or vowel-consonant (VC) sequence where the consonant is either a glide (“w”, “y”) or a glottal stop, defined as a brief silence interrupting a vowel; note that glottal stops and glides are not considered to be true consonants. Examples of level 1 utterances are “m” and “wawa”. A score of 2 is assigned to utterances containing at least one CV or VC sequence with a true consonant; if there are two or more syllables, the consonants may be the same ones or differ only in whether they are voiced or not. Examples are “bapa” and “dida”. A score of 3 is assigned to utterances containing at least two true consonants produced in different parts of the mouth and/or with different airflow characteristics. Examples are “gaba”, and “adap”. The three scores thus express the progression from motorically and linguistically simple to more complex skills. MBL scores in the BBC treatment and control cohort were compared to MBL scores in TD children at equivalent ages (Morris, 2010). Whereas the MBL is applied to babble, which does not convey lexical meaning, an equivalent measure called Syllable Structure Level (SSL) exists for meaningful speech (Paul & Jennings, 1992), and as for the MBL, we used this measure in tandem with typical norms (Morris, 2010). The source materials for the MBL and SSL are daylong naturalistic audio recordings captured with a passive, wearable audio recorder (LENA Research Foundation, Boulder, CO). The recordings are obtained in the natural environment of the families and children. The recorder is returned to the research labs and processed offline to obtain the raw, daylong audio file, which provided the source for the present MBL and SSL scores. Other measures based on the recordings will be reported in the future, including estimates of child volubility such as syllables per hour and vocalizations per hour, speech production and discourse characteristics of the target child and other family members, and other variables known to be important for language and communication development in children. All measures collected in this way are objective and algorithm-driven. For purposes of the present work, we extracted multiple 5-minute audio segments with the highest occurrence of child utterances in order to transcribe these utterances into the International Phonetic Alphabet and to compute MBL and SSL scores for each child and month.

Second, we use the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories 2 (MBCDI-2) (Fenson et al., 2007) to capture early expressive and receptive vocabulary sizes as reported by the parents when filling out the MBCDI-2 protocol forms. MBCDI-2 questionnaires were collected from each family at regular intervals (ages 12, 15, 18, 21, and 24 months), and computed percentile scores were compared between the treated and untreated participants. Percentiles were based on the publisher’s norms, separately for boys and girls. Here, we focus on receptive vocabulary, available up to age 15 months, and expressive vocabulary, available for all reported ages but regarding our pilot sample, only through age 21 months.

Third, the Ages and Stages Questionnaires - 3 (ASQ3) parent questionnaires (Squires & Bricker, 2009) capture communication abilities and personal-social development of young children. The evaluation of communication abilities differs from the expressive and receptive vocabulary checks in the MBCDI-2 in that it samples broader communication abilities, as relevant for child age, such as comprehending phrases, using language to achieve a goal, and producing multi-word sentences. The personal-social development component queries skills in activities of daily living such as feeding and dressing oneself and social interactions, both verbal and nonverbal, such as giving/receiving objects, asking for help, and role-play, as relevant for age. The ASQ3 is considered valid and reliable for purposes of tracking typical development in these and three other areas (gross motor, fine motor, and problem solving) and for identifying areas of concern, where the problem solving component is an estimate of cognitive ability (Schonhaut et al., 2013). All five questionnaire topics are applicable to children with CG because of the known risks for deficits in each of these areas (Antshel et al., 2004, Karadag et al., 2013, Potter et al., 2013). Each component is scored by summing the raw scores, then assigning one of three categories, based on the provided norms: Above cutoff (“On schedule”), close to cutoff (“Provide learning activities and monitor”), or below cutoff (“Assess further”). The ASQ3 questionnaires are available in 21 separate, age-appropriate and age-normed sets from ages 2 to 60 months. We administer this tool in 6-months intervals and report here on ages 12 and 18 months.

Data analysis

Results and trends are presented descriptively, supported with graphs. Because of the small sample size of this pilot study, statistical tests for group differences and other inferential statistical procedures were not possible here, but will be used in future reports of the study.

Quality control

Questionnaire data from the MBCDI-2 and ASQ3 were entered into the database by teams of at least two trained research assistants. The MBL and SSL scores were computed by three trained research assistants; 15% of their scores were double-scored by other team members, and differences of over 10% in a given child’s MBL score will be resolved by consensus; however, such a discrepancy has not occurred in the pilot data.

Results

Mean Babbling Level (MBL)

MBL scores of the children with CG in the treatment cohort consistently exceeded those of the control child with CG and typical control children without CG (Figure 1). For the ages for which data were available for nearly all children (11 through 21 months), the average difference between the treatment cohort and the control child with CG was 1.01, indicating that the children with CG in the treatment cohort obtained substantially higher MBL scores than the children in the other two categories. For the ages for which data were available for the control child with CG and the typical control children without CG (12, 15, 16, 18, and 20 months, listed in Morris, 2010), the average difference was -0.04, indicating that the control child with CG obtained MBL scores roughly equivalent to the typical controls without CG, but note the declining trend for the control child at the most recent ages. The difference between the children in the treatment cohort and the typical children was -0.77, indicating that the children in the treatment cohort outpaced the typical children on average. Figure 1 shows MBL scores for all children and all available ages.

26ba73a1-c10a-4741-b13c-1f647d4f822b_figure1.gif

Figure 1. Mean Babbling Level (MBL) scores for the four children with CG in the treatment cohort (blue symbols), the control child with CG (green symbols), and typically developing children (black symbols).

Syllable Structure Level (SSL)

Similar to the MBL, the SSL is calculated on the basis of 50 utterances, but unlike the MBL, it is based on meaningful speech. For one child in the treatment cohort, CG1, not enough meaningful utterances could be identified in any of the recording sessions up to 18 months, when the child left the study, to compute the SSL. Of the remaining children and for the available ages at 12, 13, 15, 19, and 20 months, the children in the treatment cohort with CG outperformed the control child with CG by 1.0. For ages 15 and 19 months, the only ages for which data were available for the treatment cohort and the typical children without CG, the children in the treatment group outperformed the typical children by 0.6. The control child with CG and the typical children without CG could not be compared directly because data at the same ages were not available, but at ages 20 and 21 months, his scores appear lower than the typical controls’ scores at age 19 and 22 months. Note that, with only one exception, the highest scores were obtained by CG2 and CG5. Figure 2 summarizes the SSL scores.

26ba73a1-c10a-4741-b13c-1f647d4f822b_figure2.gif

Figure 2. Syllable Structure Level (SSL) scores for the four children with CG in the treatment cohort (blue symbols), the control child with CG (green symbols), and typically developing children (black symbols).

Expressive and receptive vocabulary

Regarding expressive vocabulary size, the control child obtained one of the two or three lowest rankings for the ages for which data were available (Figure 3). Two children in the treatment cohort, CG2 and CG5, obtained the highest percentile rankings, which, at age 21 months, were 73 and 72, respectively. Two children, CG1 and CG4, obtained low expressive vocabulary scores, with CG1’s percentile score being 1.7 at 18 months and CG4, 4.0 at 21 months. The control child, similar to CG1 and CG4 in the treatment cohort, showed declining expressive vocabulary scores, below the 8th percentile at 21 months. Note that not having any words at all at age 12 months, which was the case for CG1 and the control child, corresponds to the 25th percentile for boys and the 20th percentile for girls, but the ranking drops rapidly with age if the expressive vocabulary does not increase substantially.

26ba73a1-c10a-4741-b13c-1f647d4f822b_figure3.gif

Figure 3. Expressive vocabulary size for ages 12 through 21 months, shown as percentile rankings, for the four children with CG in the treatment cohort (blue symbols) and the control child with CG (green symbols).

Regarding receptive vocabulary, the control child obtained the second-lowest percentile ranking (Figure 4), but note that all children including the control child show typical receptive vocabulary scores at the two ages represented in the questionnaires. The lowest percentile score, 10.5, is considered low average; it was obtained by CG1 at age 12 months. All other scores are solidly within normal limits. Figure 3 and Figure 4 summarize expressive and receptive vocabulary percentiles, respectively.

26ba73a1-c10a-4741-b13c-1f647d4f822b_figure4.gif

Figure 4. Receptive vocabulary size for ages 12 and 15 months, shown as percentile rankings, for the four children with CG in the treatment cohort (blue symbols) and the control child with CG (green symbols).

Ages and Stages Questionnaires -3 (ASQ3)

The AGS3 questionnaires at 12 and 18 months indicated that three of the four children in the treatment cohort had communication and personal-social abilities as expected for age. One child in this cohort had communication abilities below expectation for age at 18 months, and personal-social abilities close to the cutoff for age expectations. The control child scored close to the cutoff for age expectation at 18 months for communication, and at 12 and 18 months for personal-social abilities.

Regarding problem solving, of the children in the treatment cohort, only CG1 and CG4 had scores that were not consistently in the range of scores expected for age, both at age 18 months, whereas the control child was close to the cutoff at both time points. Regarding fine and gross motor skills, CG1 was the only child in the treatment cohort with scores that were below (fine motor) or close to (gross motor) the cutoff, whereas the control child had scores close to the cutoff in both areas at one time point and, for gross motor, at both time points. Across all areas, mainly one child in the treatment cohort (CG1) and the control child did not show developmental skills on schedule. Table 1 summarizes the ASQ3 scores for all children in the five assessed areas.

Table 1. Ages and Stages Questionnaires- 3 (ASQ3) scores for ages 12 and 18 months.

DomainCG1CG2CG4CG5CTR1
12 M18 M12 M18 M12 M18 M12 M18 M12 M18 M
CommunicationACBCACACACACACACACCC
Personal-SocialCCACACACACACACACCCCC
Problem SolvingACCCACACACBCACACCCAC
Fine MotorACBCACACACACACACACCC
Gross MotorCCACACACACACACACCCCC

Note: AC = Above cutoff (on schedule), BC = Below cutoff (assess further), CC = Close to cutoff (monitor)

Discussion

In a small sample of very young children with a known and highly predictive risk for speech and language disorders due to CG, we show that a program of preventive activities and routines, the BBC, may have beneficial effects in three important regards.

First, very early attention to speech sound production may increase children’s ability to produce more complex speech sounds in babble and speech. The children with CG in the treatment cohort obtained greater MBL scores during babble than the control child with CG as well as typical control children without CG. A similar pattern was seen for the SSL during meaningful speech. Together, these findings suggest that very early attention to speech sound production increases the ability to produce more complex speech sounds in babble and speech, in this case even beyond the expected levels of typically developing children. It is possible that MBL and SSL do not measure areas of crucial weakness in children with CG in general, but that targeting speech sound skills may give them a clear boost that may have beneficial effects on later speech and language development. Continued data collection and analysis in this population will provide conclusive evidence in this regard.

Second, the expressive vocabulary, and possibly to a lesser degree the receptive vocabulary, of children with CG may improve as a result of the BBC. Expressive vocabulary scores were completely within normal limits for two children in the treatment group, CG2 and CG5, whereas the control child with CG and two children in the treatment cohort, CG1 and CG4, scored below expectation. Given the 90% risk for language impairment in children with CG, finding two of four children in the treatment cohort with typical language skills may show the beneficial effects of the BBC program. The fact that receptive vocabulary scores were within normal limits for all children including the control child with CG may indicate that CG affects expressive language skills more than receptive language skills.

Third, there may be gains in competence in using language and nonverbal means to interact with others. Three of the children in the treatment cohort had communication skills and personal-social skills as expected for age, as measured with the ASQ3. These children may be gaining age-appropriate competence in activities of daily living as well.

Comparing the results from all measures, the two children with the overall highest scores were CG2 and CG5. They showed no evidence of deficits in any of the evaluated area; in fact, in the areas of speech sound complexity in meaningful speech and expressive vocabulary size, they scored at the top of the pilot sample. By contrast, CG1 did not produce enough meaningful speech for SSL scores, had low expressive vocabulary scores, and scored near or below the cutoff for expectation in all five developmental domains assessed with the ASQ3. CG4 had intermediate MBL and SSL scores but low expressive vocabulary scores and one low score in the AGQ3, namely in problem solving. The control child obtained some of the lowest MBL, SSL, expressive vocabulary, and ASQ3 scores. These patterns are consistent with cross-domain associations among the evaluated skills (speech, language, social interaction, cognitive ability, motor ability). Whether these patterns reflect global levels of disease severity, global benefits from the treatment, or a combination of these cannot be ascertained based on the present data.

Limitations and future directions

Because of the small sample size in this pilot study, generalizations to other children with CG are not possible. Whereas the MBCDI-2 percentiles were based on sex-adjusted norms, the other measures were not, but note that the two children with the overall highest performance were a girl and a boy. It is not possible to identify which components of the BBC had the greatest impact, if any, on speech, language, cognitive, and motor development. Many other variables in the study, for instance volubility of child vocalizations and environmental factors such as quantity of child-directed speech, remain to be analyzed, not only in this pilot cohort but also in the full set of families in this study. Longer-term outcomes in the primary and secondary outcome variables will be evaluated as we follow the children until age 4 years and evaluate a more complete spectrum of outcome variables including speech, language, cognitive and motor development, and quality of life. Most importantly, the trends toward beneficial effects of the BBC on the primary outcome variables of speech sound production and expressive language warrant appropriately powered larger clinical trials.

Data availability

Underlying data

Open Science Framework: Babble Boot Camp. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/YZHT4 (Peter, 2019)

The project contains the following underlying data files:

  • - UnderlyingData_MBL_SSL_MBCDI2.xlsx. This spreadsheet contains the MBL, SSL, and MBCDI-2 data for the participants.

The underlying data also include audio files recorded in the participants’ homes and hand-written questionnaires with identifiable information; therefore, these cannot be openly shared. De-identified questionnaire files can be made available to qualified researchers by contacting the first author at Beate.Peter@asu.edu.

Extended data

Open Science Framework: Babble Boot Camp. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/YZHT4 (Peter, 2019)

The project contains the following extended data files:

  • - Extended Data 1: Flow diagram

  • - Extended Data 2_Appendix_BBC_Program_v2

Reporting guidelines

Open Science Framework: Babble Boot Camp. https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/YZHT4 (Peter, 2019)

The project contains the following reporting guidelines:

  • - Reporting Guidelines 1: CONSORT checklist with Information to include in the abstract when reporting a pilot or feasibility trial

  • - Reporting Guidelines 2: CONSORT checklist with Information to include in the manuscript when reporting a pilot or feasibility trial

Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (CC-BY 4.0).

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 5
VERSION 5 PUBLISHED 11 Mar 2019
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Peter B, Potter N, Davis J et al. Toward a paradigm shift from deficit-based to proactive speech and language treatment: Randomized pilot trial of the Babble Boot Camp in infants with classic galactosemia [version 1; peer review: 2 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2019, 8:271 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.18062.1)
NOTE: If applicable, it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 11 Mar 2019
Views
16
Cite
Reviewer Report 31 May 2019
Barbara L. Davis, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX, USA 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 16
Summary: This study reports on results of parent training for facilitating speech and language development for four young children with galactosemia and one control child whose parents were not trained in stimulation techniques. Outcome measures were compared with "typical control children" but the ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Davis BL. Reviewer Report For: Toward a paradigm shift from deficit-based to proactive speech and language treatment: Randomized pilot trial of the Babble Boot Camp in infants with classic galactosemia [version 1; peer review: 2 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2019, 8:271 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.19753.r47831)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
Views
28
Cite
Reviewer Report 01 May 2019
Shelley Velleman,  Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT, USA 
Claudia Abbiati, Interdisciplinary PhD Program in Human Functioning and Rehabilitation Science, University of Vermont, Burlington, VT, USA 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 28
This manuscript focuses on an important and relevant issue: determining whether proactive intervention strategies are beneficial for infants at risk for later speech or language deficits associated with a specific condition (Classic Galactosemia, henceforth “CG”). This is an ambitious longitudinal project. ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Velleman S and Abbiati C. Reviewer Report For: Toward a paradigm shift from deficit-based to proactive speech and language treatment: Randomized pilot trial of the Babble Boot Camp in infants with classic galactosemia [version 1; peer review: 2 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2019, 8:271 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.19753.r46124)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 5
VERSION 5 PUBLISHED 11 Mar 2019
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.