ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Research Article

An assessment of the autism neuroimaging literature for the prospects of re-executability

[version 1; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]
PUBLISHED 24 Aug 2020
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

Abstract

Background: The degree of reproducibility of the neuroimaging literature in psychiatric application areas has been called into question and the issues that relate to this reproducibility are extremely complex. Some of these complexities have to do with the underlying biology of the disorders that we study and others arise due to the technology we apply to the analysis of the data we collect. Ultimately, the observations we make get communicated to the rest of the community through publications in the scientific literature.
Methods: We sought to perform a ‘re-executability survey’ to evaluate the recent neuroimaging literature with an eye toward seeing if our publication practices are helping or hindering the overall quest for a more reproducible understanding of brain development and aging. The topic areas examined include availability of the data, the precision of the imaging method description and the reporting of the statistical analytic approach, and the availability of the complete results. We applied the survey to 50 publications in the autism neuroimaging literature that were published between September 16, 2017 to October 1, 2018.
Results: The results of the survey indicate that for the literature examined, data that is not already part of a public repository is rarely available, software tools are usually named but versions and operating system are not, it is expected that reasonably skilled analysts could approximately perform the analyses described, and the complete results of the studies are rarely available. 
Conclusions: We have identified that there is ample room for improvement in research publication practices. We hope exposing these issues in the retrospective literature can provide guidance and motivation for improving this aspect of our reporting practices in the future.

Keywords

Reproducible Science, Neuroimaging, Autism, Data Sharing, Re-executability

Introduction

There is concern about the status of reproducibility in science in general and neuroimaging neuroscience in particular (Button et al., 2013; Gorgolewski & Poldrack, 2016). A particularly germane concern was expressed by Kapur and colleagues in lamenting: “a profusion of statistically significant, but minimally differentiating, biological findings; ‘approximate replications’ of these findings in a way that neither confirms nor refutes them” (Kapur et al., 2012). The replication of a specific finding (or reproducibility of a specific analysis), as reflected in a publication, has many details and nuances to it (Kennedy et al., 2019). Often, we are searching for the ‘generalizability’ of a finding: does the finding hold true when using ‘similar’ data and a ‘similar’ analysis. The similarity of data (or analysis) is a fuzzy concept. One could have a population with the same number of subjects with the same diagnosis, having the same mean age and same gender distribution as a target population; however, if the diagnosis in question is a ‘spectrum’-diagnosis (for example, autism, schizophrenia, depression, etc.), despite the ‘sameness’ of my sample in the aforementioned categories, the detailed nature of the characteristics of my sample in the features of the diagnosis itself can still be quite variable. At the level of a biological finding, we typically do not predicate the finding on an exact acquisition protocol, or a specific analysis protocol; rather, it is implicit in our finding that it should hold for other valid acquisitions and analyses of the reported types. There is increasing evidence that this implicit assumption of similarity, when it relates to the specific details of acquisition or analysis, does not necessarily hold (Glatard et al., 2015).

Some have argued that the starting point for the structured exploration of the generalizability of a specific finding (and thus a cornerstone to the quest for reproducibility) lies in the original finding itself being re-executable (Ghosh et al., 2017; Kennedy, 2019). Starting from the re-execution of a finding will allow for the systematic exploration of the generalizability of that finding, over changes in data and analysis. To date, when new studies find different findings from prior studies, it is too easy to simply argue that differences in the subject population or analysis workflow differences account for the discrepancy.

The potential impact of reproducibility issues become most obvious when trying to make sense of the accumulated literature on specific topic areas (Rane et al., 2015). For this reason, we have chosen a particular area, ‘autism’ as a way to focus the literature for this survey, so that the conclusions we reach can have potential specific implications for that topic area. We feel that the autism focus, however, will generate findings that will have similar implications to other psychiatric and developmental application areas.

In this paper, we: 1) develop a specification for what constitutes an assessment of the re-executability for a given publication in each of the domains of: data, software, execution environment, statistics and results; 2) codify this assessment in survey form; and 3) apply the survey to a subset of the autism neuroimaging literature published recently (~2018). From the results of this survey, we can begin to generalize the state of the re-executability of the recent autism neuroimaging literature, in order to identify trends and opportunities for the enhancement of the re-executability status in support of greater overall generalizability (and hence reproducibility) of the literature. The survey template could also be applied as part of the publication review process, in order to prospectively attempt to enhance these aspects of reproducibility.

Methods

Survey development

Following the concept of a ‘re-executable publication’ (Kennedy, 2019), in order to assess the prospects of re-execution of a given paper, we assess 1) the availability of the starting data, 2) the precision of the analysis description (both data processing and statistical assessment), and 3) the availability of the detailed complete results (in order to verify accuracy of re-execution). Regarding the ‘availability of the starting data’, we assess if the publication indicates how someone1 (other than the authors themselves) could appropriately access the data. The ‘precision of the analysis description’ ultimately asks if a reader who is reasonably skilled in the necessary domains, could precisely carry out the prescribed analysis steps. Specifically, are the software versions, operating system and complete parameters somehow made available to the reader? The ‘detailed complete results’ assesses if the publication indicates how to obtain the complete results, in order to both verify that the re-execution generates the same result and to overcome the limitations of only a selected summary being presented, which impedes a more complete meta-analysis of the literature.

In each of the three assessment areas, the survey distinguished between the theoretical potential for reproduction (such as complete descriptions of data used, software and commands executed, and statistical tests applied) and the practical potential for reproduction (whether the data is in fact accessible, whether the software is still available and will run). While the survey did not require the raters to actually reproduce the various steps, they were asked to use their professional judgement and past experience to determine the potential reproducibility. In these ‘judgement’ questions we allow responses of ‘Yes’, ‘Approximately’, ‘I’m not sure’, and ‘No’ to allow some degree of confidence in these judgements. For ‘results availability’, we coded ‘Yes’ if all of the results were indicated as being available, ‘Partially’ if some of the results were indicated as being available, and ‘No’ if none of the results were indicated as available or no indication of the results availability was provided.

Figure 1 provides an overview of the survey design.

a136f954-10bf-4329-a9d7-3d39fcb69e90_figure1.gif

Figure 1. High-level survey design.

OS, operating system.

The survey was constructed in Google Forms. The details of the logic and wording of the survey forms was piloted within our own group, and then released for public comment to the BrainHack Slack2 channel in August, 2018. The final complete (serialized) text of the survey is provided in S1 (see Extended data; Hodge et al., 2020c).

Literature identification

On January 23, 2019, the following PubMed query was executed:

  • (("autistic disorder"[MeSH Terms] OR ("autistic"[All Fields] AND "disorder"[All Fields]) OR "autistic disorder"[All Fields] OR "autism"[All Fields]) AND ("magnetic resonance imaging"[MeSH Terms] OR ("magnetic"[All Fields] AND "resonance"[All Fields] AND "imaging"[All Fields]) OR "magnetic resonance imaging"[All Fields] OR "mri"[All Fields])) AND ("2014/01/25"[PDat] : "2019/01/23"[PDat] AND "humans"[MeSH Terms])

This is the expansion of the general query for ‘autism AND MRI, qualified to select publications between 1/25/2014 - 1/23/2019 and where the MeSH term includes ‘human’. This query generated 811 resultant publications at the time of the query (see S2, Underlying data; Hodge et al., 2020a). We note that re-running the query today will generate additional results due to publications that have been added to PubMed after the search date but with publication dates within the defined range.

Survey application

Starting from the most recent publication and working backwards, we reviewed the title and abstract to verify publications that were indeed neuroimaging studies (not a case report or review), in English, related to autism and for which we could access the full text of the article. Working backwards from publication date, we selected the first 50 publications that met the above criteria. Of these 50 publications, 38 were available as free full text on PubMed, three were available as a PDF through a general Google Scholar search (publisher/author provided), two were available in PDF format from ResearchGate, and seven did not seem to be available without institutional access. One of three raters applied the survey to each of these articles. Each of the final results were reviewed by one rater and consensus reached with original rater if discrepancies were found.

Results

Literature selected

The final set of publications used in this report is tabulated in Table 1. The publication dates span from September 16, 2017 to October 1, 2018. Publications from 27 different journals are included. The publications selected covered a number of different MRI-based techniques (structural N=20, task-based fMRI N=14, resting-state fMRI N=13, diffusion MRI N=11 and magnetic resonance spectroscopy N=5)3.

Table 1. Publications Included in the survey.

First AuthorTitleReferencePMID
Marusak HA (Marusak et al., 2018)Mindfulness and dynamic functional neural connectivity in
children and adolescents.
Behav Brain Res. 2018 Jan 15;336:211-218. doi: 10.1016/
j.bbr.2017.09.010. Epub 2017 Sep 5.
28887198
Ramot M (Ramot et al., 2017)Direct modulation of aberrant brain network connectivity
through real-time NeuroFeedback.
Elife. 2017 Sep 16;6. pii: e28974. doi: 10.7554/eLife.28974.28917059
Bruno JL (Bruno et al., 2017)Longitudinal identification of clinically distinct
neurophenotypes in young children with fragile X syndrome.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2017 Oct 3;114(40):10767-10772.
doi: 10.1073/pnas.1620994114. Epub 2017 Sep 18.
28923933
Bottelier MA (Bottelier et al., 2017)Age-dependent effects of acute methylphenidate on
amygdala reactivity in stimulant treatment-naive patients with
Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder.
Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2017 Nov 30;269:36-42. doi:
10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.09.009. Epub 2017 Sep 12.
28938219
Hotier S (Hotier et al., 2017)Social cognition in autism is associated with the
neurodevelopment of the posterior superior temporal sulcus.
Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2017 Nov;136(5):517-525. doi:
10.1111/acps.12814. Epub 2017 Sep 22.
28940401
Chien YL (Chien et al., 2017)Altered white-matter integrity in unaffected siblings of
probands with autism spectrum disorders.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2017 Dec;38(12):6053-6067. doi: 10.1002/
hbm.23810. Epub 2017 Sep 20.
28940697
Braden BB (Braden et al., 2017)Executive function and functional and structural brain
differences in middle-age adults with autism spectrum
disorder.
Autism Res. 2017 Dec;10(12):1945-1959. doi: 10.1002/
aur.1842. Epub 2017 Sep 21.
28940848
Hegarty JP 2nd (Hegarty et al., 2018)A proton MR spectroscopy study of the thalamus in twins with
autism spectrum disorder.
Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2018 Feb
2;81:153-160. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.09.016. Epub 2017
Sep 21.
28941767
Joshi G (Joshi et al., 2017)Integration and Segregation of Default Mode Network
Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Transition-Age Males
with High-Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder: A Proof-of-
Concept Study.
Brain Connect. 2017 Nov;7(9):558-573. doi: 10.1089/
brain.2016.0483.
28942672
Carlisi CO (Carlisi et al., 2017)Shared and Disorder-Specific Neurocomputational
Mechanisms of Decision-Making in Autism Spectrum Disorder
and Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder.
Cereb Cortex. 2017 Dec 1;27(12):5804-5816. doi: 10.1093/
cercor/bhx265.
29045575
White T (White et al., 2018b)Paediatric population neuroimaging and the Generation R
Study: the second wave.
Eur J Epidemiol. 2018 Jan;33(1):99-125. doi: 10.1007/s10654-
017-0319-y. Epub 2017 Oct 24.
29064008
Zhang F (Zhang et al., 2018)Whole brain white matter connectivity analysis using machine
learning: An application to autism.
Neuroimage. 2018 May 15;172:826-837. doi: 10.1016/j.neur
oimage.2017.10.029. Epub 2017 Oct 25.
29079524
Stanfield AC (Stanfield et al., 2017)Dissociation of Brain Activation in Autism and Schizotypal
Personality Disorder During Social Judgments.
Schizophr Bull. 2017 Oct 21;43(6):1220-1228. doi: 10.1093/
schbul/sbx083.
29088456
Ni HC (Ni et al., 2018)Neural correlates of impaired self-regulation in male youths
with autism spectrum disorder: A voxel-based morphometry
study.
Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2018 Mar
2;82:233-241. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.11.008. Epub 2017
Nov 9.
29129723
Murakami Y (Murakami et al., 2018)Autistic traits modulate the activity of the ventromedial
prefrontal cortex in response to female faces.
Neurosci Res. 2018 Aug;133:28-37. doi: 10.1016/
j.neures.2017.11.003. Epub 2017 Nov 12.
29141188
Balci TB (Balci et al., 2018)Broad spectrum of neuropsychiatric phenotypes associated
with white matter disease in PTEN hamartoma tumor
syndrome.
Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2018
Jan;177(1):101-109. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32610. Epub 2017
Nov 20.
29152901
Naaijen J (Naaijen et al., 2018)Striatal structure and its association with N-Acetylaspartate
and glutamate in autism spectrum disorder and obsessive
compulsive disorder.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018 Jan;28(1):118-129. doi:
10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.11.010. Epub 2017 Nov 21.
29169826
Abbott AE (Abbott et al., 2018)Repetitive behaviors in autism are linked to imbalance of
corticostriatal connectivity: a functional connectivity MRI
study.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018 Jan 1;13(1):32-42. doi:
10.1093/scan/nsx129.
29177509
White T (White et al., 2018a)Automated quality assessment of structural magnetic
resonance images in children: Comparison with visual
inspection and surface-based reconstruction.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2018 Mar;39(3):1218-1231. doi: 10.1002/
hbm.23911. Epub 2017 Dec 5.
29206318
Wei L (Wei et al., 2018)Aberrant development of the asymmetry between
hemispheric brain white matter networks in autism spectrum
disorder.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018 Jan;28(1):48-62. doi:
10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.11.018. Epub 2017 Dec 7.
29224969
Wadsworth HM (Wadsworth et al., 2018)Action simulation and mirroring in children with autism
spectrum disorders.
Behav Brain Res. 2018 Apr 2;341:1-8. doi: 10.1016/
j.bbr.2017.12.012. Epub 2017 Dec 13.
29247748
Bernas A (Bernas et al., 2018)Wavelet coherence-based classifier: A resting-state functional
MRI study on neurodynamics in adolescents with high-
functioning autism.
Comput Methods Programs Biomed. 2018 Feb;154:143-151.
doi: 10.1016/j.cmpb.2017.11.017. Epub 2017 Nov 16.
29249338
Alexander LM (Alexander et al., 2017)An open resource for transdiagnostic research in pediatric
mental health and learning disorders.
Sci Data. 2017 Dec 19;4:170181. doi: 10.1038/
sdata.2017.181.
29257126
Gibbard CR (Gibbard et al., 2018)Structural connectivity of the amygdala in young adults with
autism spectrum disorder.
Hum Brain Mapp. 2018 Mar;39(3):1270-1282. doi: 10.1002/
hbm.23915. Epub 2017 Dec 19.
29265723
Dona O (Dona et al., 2017)Temporal fractal analysis of the rs-BOLD signal identifies
brain abnormalities in autism spectrum disorder.
PLoS One. 2017 Dec 22;12(12):e0190081. doi: 10.1371/
journal.pone.0190081. eCollection 2017.
29272297
Feczko E (Feczko et al., 2018)Subtyping cognitive profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorder
using a Functional Random Forest algorithm.
Neuroimage. 2018 May 15;172:674-688. doi: 10.1016/j.neur
oimage.2017.12.044. Epub 2017 Dec 21.
29274502
Ciaramidaro A (Ciaramidaro et al., 2018)Transdiagnostic deviant facial recognition for implicit negative
emotion in autism and schizophrenia.
Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. 2018 Feb;28(2):264-275. doi:
10.1016/j.euroneuro.2017.12.005. Epub 2017 Dec 21.
29275843
Ktena SI (Ktena et al., 2018)Metric learning with spectral graph convolutions on brain
connectivity networks.
Neuroimage. 2018 Apr 1;169:431-442. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroi
mage.2017.12.052. Epub 2017 Dec 24.
29278772
Hu Y (Hu et al., 2018)The neural substrates of procrastination: A voxel-based
morphometry study.
Brain Cogn. 2018 Mar;121:11-16. doi: 10.1016/
j.bandc.2018.01.001. Epub 2018 Jan 6.
29309854
Kohls G (Kohls et al., 2018)Altered reward system reactivity for personalized
circumscribed interests in autism.
Mol Autism. 2018 Jan 30;9:9. doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0195-
7. eCollection 2018.
29423135
Boets B (Boets et al., 2018)Alterations in the inferior longitudinal fasciculus in autism
and associations with visual processing: a diffusion-weighted
MRI study.
Mol Autism. 2018 Feb 8;9:10. doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-
0188-6. eCollection 2018.
29449909
Stivaros S (Stivaros et al., 2018)Randomised controlled trial of simvastatin treatment for
autism in young children with neurofibromatosis type 1
(SANTA).
Mol Autism. 2018 Feb 22;9:12. doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-
0190-z. eCollection 2018.
29484149
Floris DL (Floris et al., 2018)Network-specific sex differentiation of intrinsic brain function
in males with autism.
Mol Autism. 2018 Mar 6;9:17. doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-
0192-x. eCollection 2018.
29541439
Adamson K (Adamson & Troiani 2018)Distinct and overlapping fusiform activation to faces and
food.
Neuroimage. 2018 Jul 1;174:393-406. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroim
age.2018.02.064. Epub 2018 Mar 22.
29578027
Li SJ (Li et al., 2018)Alterations of White Matter Connectivity in Preschool Children
with Autism Spectrum Disorder.
Radiology. 2018 Jul;288(1):209-217. doi: 10.1148/
radiol.2018170059. Epub 2018 Mar 27.
29584599
Sen B (Sen et al., 2018)A general prediction model for the detection of ADHD and
Autism using structural and functional MRI.
PLoS One. 2018 Apr 17;13(4):e0194856. doi: 10.1371/
journal.pone.0194856. eCollection 2018.
29664902
Tsoi L (Tsoi et al., 2018)Neural substrates for moral judgments of psychological
versus physical harm.
Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2018 May 1;13(5):460-470. doi:
10.1093/scan/nsy029.
29718384
Guzman GEC (Guzman et al., 2018)Identification of alterations associated with age in the
clustering structure of functional brain networks.
PLoS One. 2018 May 24;13(5):e0195906. doi: 10.1371/
journal.pone.0195906. eCollection 2018.
29795565
Karahanoƒülu FI (Karahanoğlu et al., 2018)Diffusion-weighted imaging evidence of altered white matter
development from late childhood to early adulthood in
Autism Spectrum Disorder.
Neuroimage Clin. 2018 Jun 7;19:840-847. doi: 10.1016/
j.nicl.2018.06.002. eCollection 2018.
29946509
Zhao G (Zhao et al., 2018)Reduced structural complexity of the right cerebellar cortex
in male children with autism spectrum disorder.
PLoS One. 2018 Jul 11;13(7):e0196964. doi: 10.1371/journal.
pone.0196964. eCollection 2018.
29995885
Yan W (Yan et al., 2018)Aberrant hemodynamic responses in autism: Implications for
resting state fMRI functional connectivity studies.
Neuroimage Clin. 2018 Apr 13;19:320-330. doi: 10.1016/
j.nicl.2018.04.013. eCollection 2018.
30013915
Kim N (Kim et al., 2018)Aberrant Neural Activation Underlying Idiom Comprehension
in Korean Children with High Functioning Autism Spectrum
Disorder.
Yonsei Med J. 2018 Sep;59(7):897-903. doi: 10.3349/
ymj.2018.59.7.897.
30091324
Duret P (Duret et al., 2018)Gyrification changes are related to cognitive strengths in
autism.
Neuroimage Clin. 2018 Aug 4;20:415-423. doi: 10.1016/
j.nicl.2018.04.036. eCollection 2018.
30128280
Na S (Na et al., 2018)White matter network topology relates to cognitive flexibility
and cumulative neurological risk in adult survivors of pediatric brain tumors.
Neuroimage Clin. 2018 Aug 10;20:485-497. doi: 10.1016/
j.nicl.2018.08.015. eCollection 2018.
30148064
Chin R (Chin et al., 2018)Recognition of Schizophrenia with Regularized Support
Vector Machine and Sequential Region of Interest Selection
using Structural Magnetic Resonance Imaging.
Sci Rep. 2018 Sep 14;8(1):13858. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-
32290-9.
30218016
Gertsvolf N (Gertsvolf et al., 2018)Association between Subcortical Morphology and Cerebral
White Matter Energy Metabolism in Neonates with Congenital
Heart Disease.
Sci Rep. 2018 Sep 19;8(1):14057. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-
32288-3.
30232359
Gray JC (Gray et al., 2018)No evidence for morphometric associations of the amygdala
and hippocampus with the five-factor model personality traits
in relatively healthy young adults.
PLoS One. 2018 Sep 20;13(9):e0204011. doi: 10.1371/
journal.pone.0204011. eCollection 2018.
30235257
Vavla M (Vavla et al., 2018)Functional and Structural Brain Damage in Friedreich's Ataxia.Front Neurol. 2018 Sep 6;9:747. doi: 10.3389/
fneur.2018.00747. eCollection 2018.
30237783
Mann C (Mann et al., 2018)The effect of age on vertex-based measures of the grey-white
matter tissue contrast in autism spectrum disorder.
Mol Autism. 2018 Oct 1;9:49. doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0232-
6. eCollection 2018.
30302187

Survey application

A high-level summary of the survey results is represented in Figure 2. The complete set of question-by-question results are provided in S3 (see Underlying data; (Hodge et al., 2020c).

a136f954-10bf-4329-a9d7-3d39fcb69e90_figure2.gif

Figure 2. Survey results summary.

The 50 publications are summarized on the main factors of data availability, software specification, statistical specification and results availability.

Discussion

The recent past literature of autism neuroimaging presents a somewhat consistent picture with respect to the prospects of re-executability with regard to the characteristics we examined in this report.

Publication availability: 38 of the 50 (76%) publications appear to have ‘free full text’ available, according to the PubMed search. Of these, 33 are indexed in PubMed Central. Overall, 43 were freely available through either PubMed Central, Google Scholar or publisher or other websites.

Data availability: 16 of the 50 (32%) publications make reference to the availability of the data used in the publication. However, the publications that indicate availability are reusing data from the large repositories, whereas the publications that do not indicate data availability are principally locally conducted studies. Thus, this indicates that a large fraction of the data being used in publications are not available to the community. For the data that is available, the following resources are indicated: ABIDE 1 (Di Martino et al., 2014), ABIDE 2 (Di Martino et al., 2017), FCP/INDI (Mennes et al., 2013), COINS (Scott et al., 2011), LORIS (Das et al., 2012), NITRC (Kennedy et al., 2016), Preprocessed Connectomes Project (Puccio et al., 2016), UKBiobank (Miller et al., 2016), Brain Genomics Superstruct Project (Holmes et al., 2015), ADHD-200 (HD-200 Consortium et al., 2012), and Human Connectome Project (Glasser et al., 2016).

Image analysis: Virtually all of the publications surveyed indicate the imaging analysis software used (44 of 50, 88%). Most publications indicate the use of multiple tools. However, specific tool versions are indicated only about half of the time. While this may seem a minor point, software version can make a difference in results (Glatard et al., 2015; Ghosh et al., 2017). In this collection of 50 papers, 35 different publicly released tools (plus a number of in-house packages) are used. Not surprisingly, the following tools are used in over 10 publications each: SPM (Ashburner et al., 1998), FSL (Jenkinson et al., 2012), and FreeSurfer (Makris et al., 2003). The specific operating system used is rarely reported (1 of 50, 2%). Overall, our raters felt that in 80% of the publications a skilled image analyst could (or might be able to) repeat the analysis.

Statistical analysis: In approximately two thirds of the publications (66%), the statistical software is indicated, again with variable indication of version and no reporting of the operating system upon which the software was running. In summary, our raters felt that in 29 of the 50 papers (58%), a skilled statistical analyst could (or might be able to) repeat the analysis.

There is a distinct difference between the theoretical and practical ability to reproduce both the image analysis and statistical analysis. While the software packages used for the image analyses are specified in nearly all the cases, only some give the rater confidence that a skilled analyst could actually re-execute the analysis. Similarly, for the cases where the statistical tools are specified, only a handful are descriptive enough to give confidence in re-executability.

Results availability: Availability of the detailed results is fairly rare. All or partial results are available in seven of the 50 publications (14%). Lack of results availability causes a number of problems. Primarily, it is harder to confirm replication (or the degree to which replication was or was not achieved) without the complete set of reported observations, not just the summary tables or figures. Resorting to visual interpretations of ‘similarity’ of published figures remains fraught with issues that can hamper true understanding of new results compared to prior results. Lack of detailed results sharing also compromises subsequent meta-analytic studies that would strive to integrate observations across multiple publications. Finally, lack of complete results exacerbates the publication bias (Jennings & Van Horn, 2012) through focus on the (relatively few) statistically significant observations while not reporting the large set of observations that are not significant.

Other observations: None of the reviewed publications indicated pre-registration (Nosek et al., 2019). This is not surprising as pre-registration is a fairly new phenomenon, and its uptake in the literature can be expected to take a while. However, as a ‘baseline’ observation, it is still important to note, so that changes in the prevalence of the pre-registration practice can be monitored.

Limitations

The scope of our survey was rather limited; only 50 publications, and in a selected topic area, autism. However, as a retrospective starting point for evaluation, we believe that it fairly represents the qualitative impressions that investigators have about the nature of neuroimaging publications. We covered numerous neuroimaging subdomains: structural, diffusion, functional; and data and analytic practices in these subdomains can be rather variable.

The raters (DNK, CH, SMH) we used had over 15 years of neuroimaging research experience each; however, the specialties of each varied from more methodological/statistical to image analytic. This ‘background’ can influence the interpretation of how successfully other ‘reasonably skilled’ investigators could re-execute a given analysis. Familiarity with particular methods can both increase perceived confidence with its reuse (“Of course, everyone knows how to execute that common method”) or decrease confidence (“There are so many details that I know could be varied, how do I know what was really done?”).

Finally, the assessment of each publication is performed on the accessible manuscript as published. It is possible that data and results sharing can have occurred after publication, but this fact may not be represented in the materials reviewed. Indeed, it would be a valuable service to facilitate a more prospective management of these critical re-execution factors that can support authors in making additional supporting data and methods available post publication.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we feel that the survey results presented here reflect a state of neuroimaging publication practices that leaves ample room for improvement. While reuse of existing data is good, the majority of new data being collected for use in publications is not made publicly available. While the listing of software used is good, important details for reproducibility, such as version, detailed parameters, operating system, etc. are not fully disclosed. Similarly, statistical assessment details are variably reported, making re-execution problematic and approximate. Finally, as very little of the complete results of a publication are disclosed, assessment of the similarity of future replication attempts is severely hampered. Given the overall state of uncertainty about how reproducible (and representative) specific neuroimaging findings are, it seems prudent to begin to tighten up the variables that we as authors do have in order to better support the effective accumulation of knowledge about conditions we study. Promoting best practices in ethical data sharing, complete analytic approach disclosure, and complete results reporting seem to be critical in integrating the complex set of observations we collectively have published about the brain and how it develops and ages. The implications of these observations are that authors should redouble their efforts to be comprehensive in their reporting, even after the publication, to make as accessible as possible the detailed methods and results that they are reporting on. Specifically, authors, reviewers and editors should insist on the complete declaration of: data source and availability status, all software and versions used for data analysis and statistical assessment, the operating system (and version) for data and statistical analysis, and the disposition of the analytic results. Such a ‘checklist’ would be a valuable asset for the community and will be the subject of future efforts. This future checklist should be developed in conjunction with journal specific guidelines, and other checklists (established in conjunction with the COBIDAS report (Nichols et al., 2017), statistical reporting (Dexter & Shafer (2017), Nature Neuroscience Reporting Checklist, etc.). In such a way, publishers, editors and reviewers can impart more influence in the manuscripts that they encounter, in an effort to increase the transparency and completeness of the published record that they are playing their role in creating. Together, we hope that we can move the field forward and generate a literature that is more amenable to supporting the understanding of how our collective observations fit together in supporting the understanding of the brain.

Data availability

Underlying data

NITRC: CANDI Neuroimaging Access Point: S2_Raw_pubmed_Query_result.csv. http://doi.org/10.25790/bml0cm.68 (Hodge et al., 2020a)

This project contains the following underlying data:

  • - S2_Raw_pubmed_Query_result.csv (complete PubMed query result from 1/23/2019)

NITRC: CANDI Neuroimaging Access Point: S3_CompleteSurveyData.xlsx. http://doi.org/10.25790/bml0cm.67 (Hodge et al., 2020b)

This project contains the following underlying data:

  • - S3_CompleteSurveyData.xlsx (complete survey results)

Extended data

NITRC: CANDI Neuroimaging Access Point: S1_Prospects for Reproducibility Check List_V2 - Google Forms.pdf http://doi.org/10.25790/bml0cm.66 (Hodge et al., 2020c)

This project contains the following extended data:

  • - S1_Prospects for Reproducibility Check List_V2 - Google Forms.pdf (complete survey form)

Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (CC-BY 4.0).

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 2
VERSION 2 PUBLISHED 24 Aug 2020
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Hodge SM, Haselgrove C, Honor L et al. An assessment of the autism neuroimaging literature for the prospects of re-executability [version 1; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2020, 9:1031 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.25306.1)
NOTE: If applicable, it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 24 Aug 2020
Views
14
Cite
Reviewer Report 22 Sep 2020
Travis Riddle, Data Science and Sharing Team, Intramural Research Program, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA 
Adam G. Thomas, Data Science and Sharing Team, Intramural Research Program, National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA 
Approved
VIEWS 14
This paper highlights an important concern regarding the quality of the science seen in published literature. We applaud the authors for undertaking this work, and agree with their general conclusions that there are many opportunities for researchers to improve their ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Riddle T and Thomas AG. Reviewer Report For: An assessment of the autism neuroimaging literature for the prospects of re-executability [version 1; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2020, 9:1031 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.27929.r70151)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 04 Mar 2021
    David Kennedy, Eunice Kennedy Shriver Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, 01655, USA
    04 Mar 2021
    Author Response
    We would like to thank the reviewer for their thoughtful comments and attention to this manuscript. Here we outline our responses to the comments, and indicate where in the manuscript ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 04 Mar 2021
    David Kennedy, Eunice Kennedy Shriver Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, 01655, USA
    04 Mar 2021
    Author Response
    We would like to thank the reviewer for their thoughtful comments and attention to this manuscript. Here we outline our responses to the comments, and indicate where in the manuscript ... Continue reading
Views
21
Cite
Reviewer Report 22 Sep 2020
Karsten Specht, Department of Biological and Medical Psychology, University of Bergen, Bergen, Norway 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 21
Summary
The article by Hodge and co-workers summarises an attempt in assessing the possibility to replicate 50 published neuroimaging studies on autism. The results indicate that the majority of the studies provide only partial information that would be required ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Specht K. Reviewer Report For: An assessment of the autism neuroimaging literature for the prospects of re-executability [version 1; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2020, 9:1031 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.27929.r70168)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 04 Mar 2021
    David Kennedy, Eunice Kennedy Shriver Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, 01655, USA
    04 Mar 2021
    Author Response
    We would like to thank the reviewer for their thoughtful comments and attention to this manuscript. Here we outline our responses to the comments, and indicate where in the manuscript ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 04 Mar 2021
    David Kennedy, Eunice Kennedy Shriver Center, Department of Psychiatry, University of Massachusetts Medical School, Worcester, 01655, USA
    04 Mar 2021
    Author Response
    We would like to thank the reviewer for their thoughtful comments and attention to this manuscript. Here we outline our responses to the comments, and indicate where in the manuscript ... Continue reading

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 2
VERSION 2 PUBLISHED 24 Aug 2020
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.