ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Research Article
Revised

Eating versus skipping breakfast has no discernible effect on obesity-related anthropometric outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis

[version 2; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]
PUBLISHED 15 Oct 2020
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

This article is included in the Agriculture, Food and Nutrition gateway.

Abstract

Background: Eating or skipping breakfast for weight interests scientific and lay communities. Our objective was to systematically review and meta-analyze causal effects of eating versus skipping breakfast on obesity-related anthropometric outcomes in humans.
Methods: Six databases were searched for obesity- and breakfast-related terms (final search: 02 JAN 2020). Studies needed to isolate eating versus skipping breakfast in randomized controlled trials. Mean differences were synthesized using inverse variance random effects meta-analysis for each outcome. Positive estimates indicate higher outcomes in breakfast conditions (e.g., weight gain). Leave-one-out analysis for sensitivity and a secondary baseline habit-by-breakfast assignment analysis were performed. Risk of bias was assessed using the Cochrane risk of bias tool.
Results: Ten articles (12 comparisons; 6d to 12wk) were included. Conditions included recommendations to eat versus skip breakfast, or provision of some or all meals. 95% confidence intervals of all main analyses included the null value of no difference for each outcome: body weight (0.17 kg [-0.40,0.73], k=12, n=487, I 2=74.5), BMI (0.07 kg/m 2 [-0.10,0.23, k=8, n=396, I 2=54.1), body fat percentage (-0.27% [-1.01,0.47], k=6, n=179, I 2=52.4), fat mass (0.24 kg [-0.21,0.69], k=6, n=205, I 2=0.0), lean mass (0.18 kg [-0.08,0.44], k=6, n=205, I 2=6.7), waist circumference (0.18 cm [-1.77,2.13], k=4, n=102, I 2=78.7), waist:hip ratio (0.00 [-0.01,0.01], k=4, n=102, I 2=8.0), sagittal abdominal diameter (0.19 cm [-2.35,2.73], k=2, n=56, I 2=0.0), and fat mass index (0.00 kg/m 2 [-0.22,0.23], k=2, n=56, I 2=0.0). Subgroup analysis showed only one statistically significant result. The interaction effect for BMI (–0.36[-0.65,-0.07]) indicates assignment to conditions consistent with baseline habits had lower BMI. Leave-one-out analysis did not indicate substantial influence of any one study.
Conclusions: There was no discernible effect of eating or skipping breakfast on obesity-related anthropometric measures when pooling studies with substantial design heterogeneity and sometimes statistical heterogeneity.
Registration: PROSPERO CRD42016033290.

Keywords

Breakfast, skipping, obesity, weight, meta-analysis, systematic review, randomized controlled trials

Revised Amendments from Version 1

The new version of the article includes updated data for one of the included studies, and thus updated analyses, figures, and tables; subgroup analysis of assigned-by-baseline breakfast habits; and updated references and discussion. In addition, we updated the manuscript in accordance with reviewer comments, including conducting a formal subgroup analysis of comparing participant baseline breakfast habits with their assigned conditions. This additional analysis is briefly described in the manuscript, as well as more fully documented in the updated online materials posted in Zenodo.

See the authors' detailed response to the review by Nerys Astbury
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Enhad A. Chowdhury

Introduction

Whether one should eat or skip breakfast for weight control or loss is a topic of continued interest in both the scientific and lay communities. In 20131, we documented how breakfast eating versus breakfast skipping served as an example of how beliefs about diet can go beyond the evidence within and beyond the scientific community. The evidence at the time was dominated by over 90 observational studies – most cross-sectional – leading us to conclude that eating versus skipping breakfast as a strategy for weight was a presumption: a belief “held to be true for which convincing evidence does not yet confirm or disprove their truth”2,3. The limited scientific evidence on the topic has been translated directly to the public. For instance, we noted in our prior paper that the website of the Dr. Oz Show included an article stating, “The fact is, when you’re trying to lose body fat, you can’t skip breakfast”4. More recently, Dr. Oz himself stated, "I think for 2020, the first thing I’m going to do is ban breakfast”5, and using the social media hashtag of #TeamNoBreakfast. Meanwhile, continued scientific interest in the topic is evidenced by many more cross-sectional observational and other studies having been published; more recent narrative review articles summarizing existing literature on the topic6,7; a meta-analysis evaluating breakfast eating versus skipping on weight8 that confirmed our prior registered preliminary analyses9,10; and another group registering an analysis similar to ours after our registration (PROSPERO CRD42018110858; subsequently published11.

With mixed messaging over time about the importance of eating or skipping breakfast for the ongoing obesity epidemic, and with continued interest in the topic both scientifically and generally, it is important to synthesize the causal evidence on the effect of breakfast eating versus skipping on obesity and related outcomes, rather than relying on weaker study designs or popular opinion.

Since our earlier summaries, additional RCTs have been conducted and published (as reviewed herein). Herein, we extend our prior work to synthesize causal evidence from RCTs on eating versus skipping breakfast in humans on all reported obesity-related anthropometric outcomes we were able to extract from relevant literature.

Methods

Registration

Our study was registered with the PROSPERO international prospective register of systematic reviews (CRD42016033290) on 21 JAN 2016. The initial registration limited papers up to the registration date; however, because of the time between initial registration and this manuscript, the search was updated to 02 JAN 2020 (see Search and review strategy, below). Earlier versions of this work were published as abstracts for the American Society for Nutrition’s Annual Meeting and Scientific Sessions9,10.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Inclusion criteria were:

  • the study had at least one breakfast skipping condition and one breakfast eating condition regardless of modality (e.g., whether recommended or provisioned);

  • the study was a randomized, controlled trial (RCT);

  • study length (i.e., time on intervention) was greater than 72 hr;

  • participants were normal weight or greater, as defined by original study authors, who did not have diseases that influence weight; and

  • the study reported weight or other anthropometric outcomes.

Studies were excluded if:

  • participants had diseases or conditions that affected weight except for obesity, diabetes, and CVD;

  • breakfast eating versus breakfast skipping were confounded with other intervention components (e.g., study designs that altered intake to ensure individual weight maintenance; multicomponent behavioral interventions including breakfast without matching those components in the skipping group; or enforcing time restricted feeding apart from breakfast skipping).

Search and review strategy

Our first search was completed on 20 JAN 2016, the search refreshed on 26 JAN 2017, and the search finalized on 02 JAN 2020, with results from prior searches being deduplicated from subsequent searches.

In all search phases, searches were executed by using the application programming interfaces (APIs) of AltHealthWatch, CINAHL, Proquest Theses and Dissertations Global, PsycInfo, and Scopus using R (version 3.5.2). The following was used to search Scopus, with analogous search strategies adapted for the other databases:

  • TITLE-ABS-KEY((Obesity OR obese OR adipose OR adiposity OR overweight* OR "over weight*" OR "weight gain*" OR "weight reduc*" OR "weight los*" OR "weight maint*" OR "weight decreas*" OR "weight control*" OR "weight restrict*" OR "BMI" OR "FMI" OR "BMIz" OR "zBMI" OR "weight percentile" OR "gestational weight" OR "weight for height" OR "waist circumference" OR "skinfold thickness" OR "body composition" OR "body size" OR "fat mass" OR "body fat" OR "body mass" OR "body weight" OR "bodyweight" OR "waist hip ratio") AND (breakfast OR "break fast" OR "morning fasting" OR "morning meal")) AND DOCTYPE(ar OR ip) AND SRCTYPE(j)

Search results across databases were compared for duplication, including by title, abstract, and PubMed ID. Studies with titles and abstracts addressing animals that did not also include words related to human subjects were excluded programmatically. Titles and abstracts were then coded independently by at least two authors for inclusion/exclusion criteria. If both authors excluded a study for violation of any inclusion or exclusion criterion, it was excluded; if at least one did not exclude it, the paper was passed on for full text review.

Meta-analysis

All data and code used to estimate effect sizes and meta-analyses are provided as Extended data at https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.404139212. Additional details are included as comments within the code, including exact approaches to estimating each effect size within a study.

Effect sizes comparing breakfast eating versus skipping on each outcome were calculated for each study. Each effect size was calculated as a difference-in-difference in the native units of the outcome (e.g., kg for weight). Only outcomes for which there was more than one effect size were meta-analyzed: body weight, BMI, body fat percentage, fat mass, lean mass, fat free mass, adipose tissue mass, waist circumference, waist:hip ratio, fat mass index, sagittal abdominal diameter, and lean tissue mass. Lean mass, fat-free mass, and lean tissue mass were meta-analyzed together as ‘lean mass’; fat mass and adipose tissue mass were meta-analyzed together as ‘fat mass’. Total body water percentage and muscle mass are both reported only in Ogata et al.13; although muscle mass as an outcome was excluded, Ogata et al. also reported lean mass, which is captured in the pooled lean mass analysis.

Farshchi et al.14 reported pre and post means and standard deviations separately for each treatment period in a two-arm cross-over design. Although the unbiased estimate of the difference-in-difference was calculable from the pre and post means in each condition, the lack of information on the correlation of change within or between conditions precluded us from directly calculating the variance of the effect. We requested summaries from the authors, but the authors informed us they no longer had the raw data given that the paper was published in 2005. Thus, within-condition and between-condition correlations had to be estimated. Sievert et al.8 used a correlation coefficient of 0.3 for post-only values. We chose to estimate within-period change scores based on the within-condition correlation coefficients we estimated from Geliebter et al.15 because Geliebter et al. had all values needed to estimate within-condition, pre-post correlation coefficients. All correlation coefficients from Geliebter were greater than 0.99. Effect sizes were estimated for each outcome. Because Farshchi et al. reported no statistically significant results for any outcome, any statistically significant estimates were recalculated using the largest within-condition correlation that resulted in non-significant effect sizes. This approach may underestimate the variance, which would provide the study more weight in the meta-analysis; however, the leave-one-out analysis described below gives Farshchi the lowest weight possible.

Geliebter et al.15 reported three conditions: skipping, corn flakes, and oat porridge. We used the recommended method of the Cochrane Handbook, which is to “combine multiple groups that are eligible as the experimental or comparator intervention to create a single pair-wise comparison”16. Because we were interested in breakfast eating versus breakfast skipping, the two breakfast conditions were pooled together.

Leidy et al.17 also reported three conditions: skipping, a normal protein breakfast, and a high protein breakfast. We requested summaries from Leidy et al., who graciously provided us with separate group means and standard deviations for the changes. We used the recommended method of the Cochrane Handbook to combine breakfast conditions as described above.

Neumann et al.18 reported three conditions: skipping, high carbohydrate breakfast, and high protein breakfast. Again, we used the method recommended by the Cochrane Handbook to combine breakfast conditions. Neumann et al. reported individual-level data in their supplementary table. While reviewing the values in their supplement, we found some results to be implausible. We reached out to the authors, who clarified several subjects’ data. For our analysis, we used the updated values, and include the final updated dataset in our supplement.

Schlundt et al.19 reported follow-up data at 6 months, but the methods descriptions were unclear as to whether the interventions to eat or skip breakfast were continued past the 12-week intervention. Authors were contacted about this detail and for additional outcomes data at 12 weeks that were either not directly reported or reported as no significant strata (i.e., habitual breakfast eaters or skippers) or treatment effects; the authors informed us they no longer had the raw data given the study was published in 1992. We therefore chose to only use the change in body weight data from 12-weeks. Independent effect sizes were estimated for habitual breakfast eaters and habitual breakfast skippers.

Dhurandhar et al.20 reported body weight for the completers-only analysis in their paper. Because they registered their study as also measuring BMI, and because of the mention of an intention to treat analysis, we contacted the authors (one of whom, DBA, is a coauthor on the present meta-analysis), who provided us with summary data. Note that they also had a third group, in which participants received no specific breakfast eating or breakfast skipping recommendations; we limited our analysis to the intention to treat analyses of the breakfast eating and breakfast skipping groups. Independent effect sizes were estimated for habitual breakfast eaters and habitual breakfast skippers.

LeCheminant et al.21 were contacted for estimates of change over time for data in their Table 3. The authors graciously provided estimates of change within each group for each outcome. The data used herein, as shared by the authors, differs slightly from their publication because of increased precision and because of a reporting error in which percent body fat did, in fact, have a small but non-significant increase in the no breakfast group. This error does not change the results of their study, but the corrected values are used herein.

Ogata et al.13, Betts et al.22, and Chowdhury et al.23 effect sizes were calculated with routine equations.

Meta-analyses were calculated using the metafor package(version 2.1-0) in R. Each of 12 independent effects sizes (10 papers; 2 stratified by baseline habit) were included in each analysis as possible, depending on which outcomes were reported in which studies. Random effects analyses were calculated; no fixed effects analyses were calculated because design heterogeneity made the assumption of effect sizes being part of a homogenous distribution tenuous. The adjustment by Knapp and Hartung24 was used given the relatively small number of effect sizes. Two effect sizes were derived from separate papers of the Bath Breakfast Project (BBP; Betts et al. and Chowdhury et al.). Because these were independent samples (normal or with obesity) we treated them as independent even though they came from the same overarching study. Similarly, although there is plausibly some correlation amongst effect sizes calculated within the habit strata in Dhurandhar et al. and Schlundt et al. by nature of being part of the same overarching study, we treated the effect sizes as independent.

Leave-one-out analysis was used as a sensitivity analysis to investigate the influence of any single study for each outcome, in which each study was omitted from the dataset at a time, and then the meta-analysis was recalculated.

Effect estimates are displayed as mean differences with 95% confidence intervals in the native units of the outcome. I2 (%) and p-values for tests of heterogeneity are also reported. No multiple-comparison corrections are applied within or among outcomes. There are few effect sizes (k=12), there is substantial design heterogeneity (e.g., study length, types of breakfast, different populations), and there is statistical heterogeneity in several outcomes; therefore, funnel plot asymmetry is not presented because visual estimation of asymmetry is unreliable for small k25, the test is underpowered for small k26, and any association between effect size and variance may plausibly be explained by study design or other factors rather than just publication bias26.

We calculated a secondary analysis comparing habitual breakfast eaters versus habitual breakfast skippers, as defined by the authors of the published papers. This subgroup analysis completed the preliminary analyses we initially published as abstracts9,10. If baseline habits were not reported, or if authors reported that the baseline habits of the participants were mixed, the study arms were excluded. The subgroup analysis was based on the same effect size estimates generated for the primary analysis. The same functions were used in R, except for modeling the outcome variable as a function of both assigned condition and baseline breakfast habits. Additional methods can be found in the subgroup analysis document and code located in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4041392.

Risk of bias

Risk of bias was assessed independently by two investigators (MMBB/JEM for all but Ogata 2019 and MMBB/AWB for Ogata 2019) using Cochrane’s Risk of Bias Tool27. Given that the interventions are obvious to participants (eating versus skipping breakfast), we only coded blinding of personnel, and readers should be aware of the risk of non-blinded interventions. We do not use the approach of assigning a binary risk of bias to an entire study (e.g., if one criterion is high risk in a study, the entire study is considered high risk); however, we provide the individual ratings for each article and readers can apply such an approach if they wish.

Results

PRISMA diagram

The search results are shown in the PRISMA diagram in Figure 1. The results of each of the three phases of the search are shown.

b2404f73-bea5-466c-b834-4f07d2761a3f_figure1.gif

Figure 1. PRISMA diagram.

Three searches were undertaken. For searches 2 and 3, the numbers in parentheses represent unique results to that search. *Several ‘papers from other sources’ were identified in prior searches, but those papers were captured by the third search.

Inclusion table

Ten papers were included with 12 effect sizes (see Table 1 for descriptions). Briefly, of the 10 studies included: six were conducted in the United States, three in the United Kingdom, and one in Japan; two were cross-over RCTs and eight were parallel arm RCTs; length ranged from 6 days to 16 weeks; five provisioned some or all foods and five were recommendations for dietary consumption; two stratified on baseline eating or skipping habits, two included only habitual breakfast eaters, three included only habitual breakfast skippers, two reported mixed baseline habits, and one did not specify baseline habits; four reported race/ethnicity of participants; four included females only, one included males only, and five included both females and males. For breakfast definitions, dietary compositions, and timing, see Table 1 and Figure 2. Breakfast definitions and timing of consumption varied amongst the studies included and ranged from highly controlled and prescribed to broad recommendations (Figure 2).

Table 1. Included studies.

StudyLocationPopulationAge
(Mean ± SD)1
Race/Ethnicity2InterventionProvision
of Food
Baseline
Breakfast
Habits
(Eaters vs
Skippers)
Breakfast
Eating and
Breakfast
Skipping
Definitions3
Dietary
Composition3
Weight-related
anthropometric
measures
preregistered
as primary
or secondary
outcome
Weight-related
anthropometric
measures
reported4
Betts 2014UKAdults: n=33
64% Female
21 – 60 y
All: 36 ± 11 y
BF: 36 ± 11 y
Skip: 36 ± 11 y
Not reported6 wk parallel arm
RCT
Recommendation
to eat or skip
breakfast
NoMixedBreakfast
group:
consume
energy intake
of ≥700 kcal
before 1100h
daily, with
at least half
consumed
within 2 h of
waking
Fasting group
(skip): Extend
overnight fast
by abstaining
from ingestion
of energy-
providing
nutrients (plain
water only)
until 1200 h
each day.
No
recommendation
for the diet was
given.
Yes:
ISRCTN31521726
BW, BF%, BMI,
ATM, FMI, LTM,
SAD, WC, WHR
Chowdhury
2016
UKAdults: n=23
65% Female
21 – 60 y
All: 44 ± 10 y
BF: 44 ± 10 y
Skip: 44 ± 10 y
Not reported6 wk parallel arm
RCT
Recommendation
to eat or skip
breakfast
NoMixedBreakfast
group:
consume
energy intake
of ≥700 kcal
before 1100h
daily, with
at least half
consumed
within 2 h of
waking
Fasting group
(skip): Extend
overnight fast
by abstaining
from ingestion
of energy-
providing
nutrients (plain
water only)
until 1200 h
each day.
No
recommendation
for the diet was
given.
Yes:
ISRCTN31521726
BW, BF%, BMI,
ATM, FMI, LTM,
SAD, WC, WHR
Dhurandhar
2014
USAAdults: n=185
76% Female
20 – 65 y
BF:
40.6 ± 12.0 y
Skip:
42.0 ± 12.4 y
Total: WHN: 93,
BNH:74, WH:17,
BH:8, O:12
Breakfast:
WHN: 45,
BNH:40, WH:5,
BH:5, O:6
Skip:
WHN: 48,
BNH:34, WH:12,
BH:3, O:6
16 wk parallel
arm RCT
Recommendation
to eat breakfast,
skip breakfast, or
neither (control
group); all three
treatment groups
were given a
USDA pamphlet
suggesting good
nutrition habits in
baseline skippers
and eaters
NoStratifiedBreakfast
Eating: meal
before 1000h.
Skipping:
no eating
or caloric
consumption
prior to 1100 h.
The breakfast
group received
the USDA
pamphlet with
a handout
instructing
participants
to consume
breakfast before
1000 h every day.
The breakfast
handout also
provided
suggestions of
food items that
might constitute
a healthy
breakfast;
however,
no specific
restrictions were
given on types of
foods that could
be consumed
for the breakfast
meal. The
skipping group
received the
USDA pamphlet
with a handout
instructing
participants
not to consume
any calories
before 1100 h
every day, and
that only water
or zero-calorie
beverages could
be consumed
from the time
of waking
until 1100 h.
No specific
composition was
recommended.
Yes:
NCT01781780
BW, BMI
Farshchi
2005
UKAdults: n=10
100% Female
19 – 38 y
Total:
25.5 ± 5.7 y
Not reported2 wk per
condition, cross-
over RCT
Intervention
program to eat or
skip breakfast
Breakfast
and one
snack
Habitual
eaters
Breakfast
between
0700h
and 0800h.
Skipping
nothing prior
to 1030 h.
Breakfast group
consumed a
pack (45 g) of
whole-grain
cereal
with 200 mL
2% milk between
0700 h and
0800 h. and
consumed a
chocolate-
covered cookie
between 1030 h
and 1100 h.
Skippers had
nothing prior
to both groups
consuming a
48-g chocolate-
covered cookie
between 1030 h
and 1100 h.
Skippers then
had the cereal
and 2%-fat milk
between 1200 h
and 1230 h. Both
groups then
consumed 2
additional meals
and 2 snacks of
content similar
to usual during
the times of
1330–1400,
1530–1600,
1800–1830, and
2030–2100.
Subjects
were asked to
consume their
main evening
meal (dinner)
between 1800
and 1830.
Not registeredBW, BF%, BMI,
WC, WHR
Geliebter
2014
USAAdults: n=36
50% Female
8 – 65 y
Total sample:
33.9 ± 7.5 y
M:35.6 ± 6.1 y
F: 32.3 ± 8.6 y
Total: W:16,
B:10, H:6, A:3,
O:3
Skip: W:4, B:3,
H:3, A:1, O:1
C: W:6,
Breakfast:3,
H:2, A:2, O:1
P: W:6, B:4, H:1,
A:0, O:1
4 wk parallel arm
RCT
Recommendation
to skip breakfast
compared
to provision
of high fiber
(oat porridge)
and non-fiber
(cornflakes)
breakfasts
Breakfast
only
Unspecified0830 h arrival
weekdays with
15 min given
to consume
breakfast or
water for skip
group.
Breakfasts
were given to
take home for
weekends with
no time given
on weekends
No
recommendation
for the remainder
of the diet was
given.
Registered after:
NCT02035150
BW, FFM, FM,
WC, WHR
LeCheminant
2017
USAAdults: n=49
100% Female
18 – 55 y
BF:
23.7 ± 7.5 y
Skip:
23.6 ± 5.0 y
Not reported4 wk parallel arm
RCT
Recommendation
to eat or skip
breakfast in
habitual skippers
NoHabitual
skippers
Breakfast
group to eat
within 1.5 h
of awakening
and consume
15% total
energy intake
for the day
by 0830 h.
Skippers were
defined as not
consuming
a snack or
meal (only
noncaloric
beverages)
until after
1130 h.
No
recommendation
for the remainder
of the diet was
given. Both
groups asked
to wake up by
0800.
Not registeredBW, FM, LM,
BF%, BMI
Leidy 2015USAAdolescent:
n=54
57% Female
19 y (mean)
Skip:19 ± 1 y
Normal Protein
BF: 18 ± 1 y
High Protein
BF: 19 ± 1 y
Total: W:33,
B:19, O:2
Skip: W:6, B:3,
O:0
Normal Protein:
W:16, B:5, O:0
High Protein:
W:11, B:11, O:2
12 wk parallel
arm RCT
Recommendation
to skip breakfast
compared to
the provision of
normal protein
and high protein
breakfasts in
habitual skippers
Breakfast
only
Habitual
Skippers
Breakfast
consuming
groups were
provided
with specific
breakfast
meals with
consumption
of breakfast
between
0600 h
and 0945 h
each day. The
skipping group
continued to
skip breakfast
(only water)
before 1000.
The NP meals
contained 15%
protein, 65%
carbohydrates,
and 20% fat
and consisted
of ready-to-eat
cereals with milk.
The HP meals
contained 40%
protein, 40%
carbohydrates,
and 20% fat
and consisted
of egg-based
pancakes and
ham; egg-based
waffles with
pork-sausage;
egg and pork
scramble; and
an egg and
pork burrito. The
breakfast meals
were provided
on a weekly
basis with meal
preparation
instructions.
Breakfasts
were 18% of
total dietary
calories. No
recommendation
for the remainder
of the diet was
given.
Not registeredBW, FM, LM,
BF%, BMI
Neumann
2016
USAAdults: n =24
100% Female
11 – 36 y
Skip:
27.1 ± 1.8 y
Carbohydrate
BF: 21.9 ±
0.9 y
Protein BF:
23.3 ± 1.3 y
Skip:
C:5, H:1, B:1,
A:0, I:1
Carbohydrate:
C:3, H:1,
B:1, A:2, I:1
Protein:
C:6, H:1, B:1,
A:0, I:0
8 d parallel arm
RCT
Assignment
to skip or eat
breakfast
with provision
(breakfast or
water) in habitual
skippers
Breakfast
only
Habitual
skippers
Breakfast
group: eat
breakfast
before or at
the start of
daily activities
and within
two hours of
waking with
consumption
typically
occurring
no later than
1000 h.
Skipping
group:
provided water
with no other
instructions
given.
Breakfast:
CHO breakfast
consisted of 1
English muffin
(57 g), yogurt
(170 g), cream
cheese (17g),
and water
(227 mL). The
PRO breakfast
consisted of
a proprietary
breakfast
sandwich
(145 g), Greek
yogurt (150g),
and water
(227 mL). Both
test breakfasts
were similar
in kilocalories
and controlled
for fat and
fiber. Skipping
group was
provided water
(227 mL). No
recommendation
for the remainder
of the diet was
given.
Not registeredBW, BMI
Ogata 2019JapanAdult: n=10
0% Female
20 – 30 y
BF to Skip:
24.8 ± 2.9 y
Skip to BF:
25.6 ± 3.0 y
Japanese:106 d per condition,
cross-over RCT
Intervention to eat
or skip breakfast
All foodHabitual
eaters
Breakfast
eating group
consumed
breakfast
at 0700 h,
breakfast
skipping
group nothing
prior to lunch
at 1230 h.
Breakfast eating
group had
33.3% of daily
energy intake
for each of the
three meals
of breakfast
(0700 h), lunch
(1230 h) and
dinner (1800 h).
The breakfast
skipping group
had 0% for
breakfast, 50%
of daily energy
intake each for
lunch (1230 h)
and dinner
(1800h). The
24-h energy
intake was equal
for both dietary
conditions. The
meals provided
were individually
adjusted (3042 ±
598 kcal/d, 14%
protein, 25%
fat, and 61%
carbohydrates).
Yes:
UMIN000032346
BW, BF%, FM,
FFM, MM, TBWP
Schlundt
1992
United
States
Adults: n= 45
100% Female
18 – 55 y
Only range
stated
Not reported12 wk parallel
arm RCT
Baseline
breakfast eaters
and skippers
were assigned
to either eat or
skip breakfast
with total diet
composition and
caloric content
same between
groups
NoStratifiedMenus and
instructions
for 3 meals
(breakfast,
lunch and
dinner) or 2
meals (lunch
and dinner),
timing not
specified in
the paper.
Total dietary
composition:
50–55% of
energy from
carbohydrates,
15–20% from
protein, and
25–30% from
fats. No-
breakfast diet
consisted of two
meals, lunch
(1672 kJ)
and supper
(3344 kJ).
Breakfast diet
consisted of
three meals,
breakfast
(1672 kJ), lunch
(1254 kJ), and
supper
(2090 kJ).
Not registeredBW

1BF, Breakfast.

2A, Asian; B, Black; BH, Black Hispanic; BNH, Black Non-Hispanic; C: Caucasian; H, Hispanic; I, Indian; O, Other; W, White; WH, White Hispanic; WNH, White Non-Hispanic.

3Definitions paraphrased from each study paper.

4ATM, adipose tissue mass; BF%, body fat percentage; BW, body weight; FFM, fat-free mass; FM, fat mass; FMI, fat mass index; LM, lean mass; LTM, lean tissue mass; MM, muscle mass; SAD, sagittal abdominal diameters; TBWP, total body water percentage; WC, waist circumference; WHR, waist:hip ratio. Some additional outcomes might have been mentioned in the paper, but quantitative results may not have been reported after the intervention.

b2404f73-bea5-466c-b834-4f07d2761a3f_figure2.gif

Figure 2. Schematic of breakfast versus skipping timing and patterns.

The top section outlines the patterns for the included studies; the middle section shows a few examples of studies we did not classify as eating versus skipping breakfast that are explained further in the ‘Notable Exclusions’ section and in Table 3; and the bottom is a legend for the figure. ‘Inferred eating window’ represents the times we inferred that participants were permitted or recommended to consume food as reported in the papers; ‘specified eating window’, ‘breakfast eating window’, and ‘assigned eating times’ were reported by the authors in either absolute or relative times (e.g., number of hours since waking). For more details for the included studies, see Table 1.

Meta-analyses of anthropometric outcomes

Figure 3 shows a composite forest plot that includes all meta-analyzable, obesity-related, anthropometric outcomes. In all cases, the 95% confidence intervals included the null of no differences between skipping and eating breakfast (frequently interpreted as “not statistically significant”). Table 2 shows the numerical estimates of the values displayed in the forest plots. Therefore, no discernible effects of breakfast eating or breakfast skipping on body weight (kg), BMI (kg/m2), body fat percentage (%), fat mass (kg), lean mass (kg), waist (cm), waist:hip ratio, sagittal abdominal diameter (cm) and fat mass index (kg/m2) were found in these primary analyses.

In the secondary analysis comparing habitual breakfast eaters versus habitual breakfast skippers (as defined by the authors of the published papers), forest plots and a summary table can be found in the subgroup between group forest plots document and subgroup between group summary table located in https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.4041392. Briefly, there was no discernible effect of stratification of baseline habits on four of the outcomes: body weight (4 effect sizes for habitual breakfast eaters; 4 effect sizes for habitual breakfast skippers), body fat percentage (2 and 2 effect sizes, respectively), fat mass (1 and 2 effect sizes, respectively), or lean mass (1 and 2 effect sizes, respectively). Insufficient study arms existed to test differences for waist circumference, waist:hip ratio, sagittal abdominal diameter, and fat mass index. Only BMI was statistically significant (2 and 4 effect sizes, respectively). The negative estimate of –0.36[-0.65,-0.07] BMI units indicates that individuals assigned to conditions consistent with their baseline habits had lower values than those assigned opposite of their habits. That is, habitual breakfast skippers had lower BMI when skipping breakfast and habitual breakfast eaters had lower BMI when eating breakfast, compared to skippers eating and eaters skipping.

b2404f73-bea5-466c-b834-4f07d2761a3f_figure3.gif

Figure 3. Composite forest plot of seven meta-analyzable anthropometric outcomes.

Sagittal abdominal diameter and fat mass index were only included in the two papers from the Bath Breakfast Project (Betts et al. and Chowdhury et al.), and are not plotted here; outcomes of muscle mass and total body water percent were only included in Ogata et al., and so no meta-analyzable estimate was possible. See Table 2 for the numerical values of these seven analyses, plus the sagittal abdominal diameter and fat mass index. Studies without point estimates and confidence intervals within an outcome indicates that the study did not report that outcome. 95% confidence intervals for individual studies and for the width of the diamond representing the summary estimate are presented. Horizontal dotted lines for the summary of the meta-analyses represents the 95% prediction interval. For the column ‘Habit’: e, habitual eaters; s, habitual skippers; u, unspecified or mixed. Values to the left of 0 indicate that the breakfast condition had a greater decrease in the outcome relative to the skipping condition. For all outcomes except lean mass, values to the left would therefore traditionally be considered 'favoring' the breakfast condition (e.g., lower body weight)

Table 2. Effect sizes for each study and meta-analyzable anthropometric outcome shown in Figure 3.

Data are presented as mean [95% CI] for each study and the summary estimate, expressed as mean difference. Positive values are higher during breakfast conditions. n represents the total number of individuals within a study; k is the number of effect sizes in a meta-analytic estimate; MD is mean difference; I2 represents heterogeneity, with the associated p-value representing the statistical test for significant heterogeneity. Outcomes of muscle mass and total body water percent were only included in Ogata et al., and so no meta-analyzable estimate was possible.

StudynBody weight
(kg)
BMIBody fat
(%)
Fat mass
(kg)
Lean mass
(kg)
Waist
circumference (cm)
Waist:hip
ratio
Sagittal
abdominal
diameter (cm)
Fat mass
index
Betts 2014330.20
[-0.46,0.86]
0.11
[-0.12,0.34]
-0.20
[-1.36,0.96]
0.00
[-0.85,0.85]
0.00
[-0.82,0.82]
-0.30
[-1.58,0.98]
0.00
[-0.02,0.02]
0.00
[-0.64,0.64]
0.01
[-0.28,0.30]
Chowdhury 2016230.80
[-0.19,1.79]
0.26
[-0.08,0.60]
-0.24
[-2.21,1.73]
-0.10
[-2.12,1.92]
0.40
[-1.63,2.43]
2.20
[-0.56,4.96]
0.02
[-0.00,0.04]
0.40
[-0.28,1.08]
-0.04
[-0.76,0.68]
Dhurandhar 2014e1090.06
[-1.68,1.80]
0.03
[-0.59,0.65]
Dhurandhar 2014s95-0.31
[-2.09,1.46]
-0.09
[-0.72,0.54]
Farshchi 200510-0.50
[-1.07,0.07]
-0.20
[-0.40,0.00]
-0.60
[-1.45,0.25]
-1.00
[-2.00,0.00]
0.00
[-0.01,0.01]
Geliebter 2014361.30
[0.46,2.14]
-0.09
[-2.38,2.19]
1.00
[-1.24,3.24]
0.85
[0.27,1.43]
0.00
[-0.02,0.02]
LeCheminant 2017490.64
[0.09,1.19]
0.24
[0.03,0.44]
0.29
[-0.17,0.75]
0.41
[-0.03,0.85]
0.06
[-0.21,0.33]
Leidy 201554-1.20
[-3.90,1.50]
-0.39
[-1.30,0.52]
-1.91
[-3.41,-0.42]
-1.77
[-3.62,0.08]
0.55
[-0.74,1.85]
Neumann 2016230.37
[-0.41,1.16]
0.17
[-0.16,0.49]
Ogata 201910-0.93
[-1.37,-0.49]
0.12
[-0.93,1.17]
0.31
[-0.43,1.05]
0.54
[-0.18,1.26]
Schlundt 1992e292.70
[-0.19,5.59]
Schlundt 1992s16-1.70
[-5.55,2.15]
MD
[CI]
0.17
[-0.40,0.73]
0.07
[-0.10,0.23]
-0.27
[-1.01,0.47]
0.24
[-0.21,0.69]
0.18
[-0.08,0.44]
0.18
[-1.77,2.13]
0.00
[-0.01,0.01]
0.19
[-2.35,2.73]
0.00
[-0.22,0.23]
k (n)12 (487)8 (396)6 (179)6 (205)6 (205)4 (102)4 (102)2 (56)2 (56)
I2 (p for I2)74.5 (<0.001)54.1 (0.036)52.4 (0.055)0.0 (0.311)6.7 (0.682)78.7 (0.002)8.0 (0.413)0.0 (0.376)0.0 (0.895)

Table 3. Notable studies that were excluded with reasons.

StudyReason for exclusion*Notes
Alwatter 201532No weight or anthropometryAdolescent girls
Frape 199733No weight or anthropometryAdults
Gwin 201834No weight or anthropometryAdults
Halsey 201235No weight or anthropometryAdults
Hoertel 201436No weight or anthropometryAdolescent girls
Leidy 201337No weight or anthropometryAdolescent girls
Reeves 201438No weight or anthropometryAdults
Reeves 201539No weight or anthropometryAdults
Rosi 201840Less than 72 hrAdult men; no weight
Yoshimura 201741Less than 72 hrAdult women; one-day study
Zakrewski-Frue 201742Less than 72 hrAdolescent girls; only baseline weight
Carlson 200743Not about breakfastAdults; did not include weight outcomes; compared 1 vs 3 meals per day with
weight being deliberately maintained (see Figure 2)
Hirsch 197529Not about breakfastAdults; dinner only versus breakfast only (see Figure 2)
Keim 199744Not about breakfastAdult Women; distribution of calories as 70% morning versus 70% evening
Tinsley 201945Not about breakfastAdult women; time-restricted feeding versus not (see Figure 2)
Wehrens 201728Not about breakfastAdult men; non-randomized order; all meals (not just
breakfast) shifted 5 hours
(see Figure 2)
Ask 200646No skipping conditionChildren; quasi-experiment
Crepinsek 200647No skipping conditionChildren
Douglas 201948No skipping conditionAdolescent girls
Jakubowicz 201249No skipping conditionAdults
Powell 199831No skipping conditionChildren
Rosado 200830No skipping conditionChildren
St Onge 201550No skipping conditionChildren
Versteeg 201751No skipping conditionAdult men
Zakrewski-Frue 201852No skipping conditionAdolescent girls; breakfast skipping was alternate day
skipping; no weight beyond baseline
Chowdhury 201953Data published elsewhereBBP: weight data in Chowdhury 2016
Gonzalez 201854Data published elsewhereBBP: weight data in Betts 2014 and Chowdhury 2016
Tuttle 195455Confounded designBoys, men, and women; non-counterbalanced cross-
over; some participants were assigned to gain or lose
weight

* Studies were excluded for at least one reason; the reasons given in this column may not be the only reason for exclusion.

Risk of bias

Risk of bias varied by study (Figure 4). Two studies had low risk of bias across all categories: Dhurandhar 2014 and Ogata 201913. Two studies, Betts 201422 and Chowdhury 201623, were coded as high risk of bias for the criterion of blinding participants and personnel because the authors clearly indicated that personnel were not blinded. Given that the interventions are obvious to participants (eating versus skipping breakfast), we only focus on blinding of personnel, and readers should be aware of the risk of non-blinded interventions. On the other hand, many of the categories in the risk of bias in each study were unclear, and it is therefore uncertain whether the risk was high or low.

b2404f73-bea5-466c-b834-4f07d2761a3f_figure4.gif

Figure 4. Risk of bias assessment.

Each included paper was assessed for risk of bias using the Cochrane Risk of Bias tool. Given that the interventions are obvious to participants (eating versus skipping breakfast), we only coded blinding of personnel, and readers should be aware of the risk of non-blinded interventions.

Sensitivity analysis: Leave-one-out analysis

The leave-one-out analysis is shown in Figure 5. Little difference is noted among the analyses, with substantial overlap of confidence intervals in all cases. When considering statistical significance (i.e., confidence intervals that do not include 0), leaving Farshchi et al.14 out of the analysis results in significantly greater BMI in the breakfast conditions than the skipping conditions. When Leidy et al.17 is excluded, fat mass is greater in the breakfast than the skipping conditions. Waist:hip ratio is centered on zero with no estimable confidence interval when Chowdhury et al.23 is left out because the other three estimates are all 0.00. We reiterate that none of these summaries took multiple comparisons into account.

b2404f73-bea5-466c-b834-4f07d2761a3f_figure5.gif

Figure 5. Leave-one-out analysis.

Within each column, the diamond represents the meta-analytic summary estimate when leaving out the study in a particular row. Row and column combinations without diamonds represent outcomes that are not reported for that particular study. *The waist:hip ratio had no estimable confidence interval because the three remaining estimates were all 0.00. Sagittal abdominal diameter and fat mass index were only included in the two papers from the Bath Breakfast Project (Betts et al. and Chowdhury et al.), and therefore a leave-one-out analysis would include only a single study; outcomes of muscle mass and total body water percent were only included in Ogata et al., and so a leave-one-out analysis is not possible.

Notable exclusions

Notable exclusions are located in Table 3. Broad areas to note are the lack of a skipping group for comparison to breakfast groups, intervention periods that were less than 72 hr in duration, studies that had the comparison of interest but did not measure body weight, and studies whose primary purpose did not isolate breakfast eating versus breakfast skipping, such as time restricted feeding and shift in consumption periods. Two examples of the latter include Wehrens et al.,28 who shifted all meals by 5 hours (as well as not being in a randomized order), to extreme time restriction of Halberg et al.29 who assigned only breakfast or dinner (Figure 2).

In this meta-analysis, our included studies were all conducted in adults/adolescents, but, as noted in Table 3, there have been several related studies conducted in children; however, none of the studies in children had a true skipping group. For instance, Rosado et al.30 had a control group with no intervention, which is not equivalent to assigning children to skip breakfast. Similarly, Powell et al.31 did have a group that was assigned to consume a slice of orange as an attention placebo control, but again the children were not assigned to otherwise skip breakfast.

Discussion

Summary

The causal effect of eating versus skipping breakfast on obesity-related anthropometric outcomes was non-significantly different from zero across body weight, BMI, body fat percentage, fat mass, lean mass, waist circumference, waist:hip ratio, sagittal abdominal diameter, and fat mass index. Our results largely match our prior analyses9,10, as well as the analysis of body weight conducted by Sievert et al.8.

The choices of inclusion/exclusion criteria, adjustments, and assumptions to use when meta-analyzing data can influence the interpretation of results, so we highlight some of our choices here. Our choice of including studies greater than 72 hours in duration (with the shortest actually included being 6 days) should avoid including experiments measuring only transient differences from hydration, glycogen, or other very acute physiological responses. Furthermore, this approach allowed us to collate all relevant experiments and enables readers to reanalyze our results with their preferred study duration cutoff should the reader so choose. We chose to define breakfast eating versus breakfast skipping according to the intentions of the original study authors where possible. This was primarily done because a common recommendation is to eat breakfast to lose weight without any other qualifications. In doing so, we included studies that 1) may or may not have been on a weight loss background, 2) included a range of weight categories, and 3) included a variety of breakfast compositions, timings, and definitions. Another limitation that was well state by one of our reviewers: “Many of the studies included in this and other SR [systematic reviews] are small, and are often conducted in participants with a mixed usual breakfast habit who are not overweight or attempting to limit their energy intake or reduce their body weight.”

While we cannot rule out that there may be some statistically significant combination of studies, subgroups, splitting-versus-pooling of different breakfasts, or different imputation strategies, we note that the results are fairly consistently centered near zero. However, Sievert et al.(8) and Bonnet et al.(11) both concluded small but statistically significant differences in favor of breakfast skipping. Each review included a different subset of studies, predominantly driven by duration of studies included. For one specific point of comparison, Sievert et al. used a different imputation strategy than we did for Farshchi et al. We estimated the correlations based on the estimates from Geliebter et al., while constraining the interval to the statistically non-significant results. Bonnet et al. did not include Farshchi et al. because it was too short (2 weeks) for their cutoff of 4 weeks.

Our leave-one-out analyses, produced only two values that became statistically significantly different in favor of skipping breakfast: BMI when Farshchi et al. was excluded. The subgroup analysis of baseline breakfast habits included few effect sizes to estimate interaction effects between baseline breakfast habits and breakfast assignment. We could test for differences in 5 of the outcomes. Of these, BMI showed a significant interaction between baseline habits and assigned condition. The statistical significance of these secondary estimates should be considered within the following contexts. In the leave-one-out analyses, the 95% confidence intervals did not differ substantially from other leave-one-out analyses. In the subgroup analyses, few effect sizes were available for comparison. There were 4 treatment effect estimates each for habitual breakfast eaters and habitual breakfast skippers for body weight, while there were only 2 and 4, respectively, for BMI. Some outcomes had only 1 and 2 effect estimates for eaters and skippers, respectively. In all cases, we did not adjust for multiple comparisons because of their exploratory nature. Given the small departure of the confidence intervals from 0, it is likely these would no longer be statistically significant if multiple comparisons were considered. Even if effects turned out to be non-zero, the 95% confidence and prediction intervals of the outcomes include effect sizes of low clinical significance, and thus further work would be needed to determine if non-zero effects are actually clinically meaningful.

Despite this relative consistency in summary effect sizes, we note that there was substantial design heterogeneity. The length of studies, for instance, varied substantially. To be confident in effects of obesity-related interventions, longer term studies are desired. However, the need for longer-term studies is often to guard against concluding that early effects (weeks to months) will result in sustained weight loss over months to years. Given the overall null findings herein, suggesting a need for longer studies would serve to test whether these relatively acute null findings reflect long-term adaptations to establishing breakfast habits. In addition, some have argued that it is not merely eating versus skipping breakfast that is important, but rather that the type of breakfast matters (c.f., Leidy et al. 20167). Such an argument does not invalidate the question asked or the findings of this meta-analysis, however. If, for instance, a breakfast of a particular characteristic is what influences weight – be it fiber content, protein, energetic load, timing from waking, or others – then the question would not be whether eating versus skipping breakfast matters; rather, research would need to test the effects of that particular breakfast versus comparator groups, whether those comparator groups be different breakfasts or no breakfast at all.

We clarify that our results are limited to obesity-related anthropometric outcomes. As stated previously, “Just because breakfast consumption may not have a statistically significant effect on weight does not make breakfast a bad recommendation”56, nor does it necessarily make it a good recommendation. Our results do not inform whether eating versus skipping breakfast is of value for blood glucose control (c.f.,57, cardiometabolic risk factors (c.f.,11,15), school performance (c.f.,58, or other outcomes; nor do our results inform the effects of eating versus skipping breakfast as part of a multicomponent behavioral/intensive lifestyle intervention or time restriction paradigm (e.g., early vs late time-restricted feeding). The inference of the results of this study are limited to the broad and simplistic recommendation to eat or skip breakfast to affect anthropometric outcomes.

Conclusion

There was no discernible effect of eating or skipping breakfast on obesity-related anthropometric measures when pooling studies with substantial design heterogeneity and sometimes statistical heterogeneity in our primary analyses.

Data availability

Underlying data

All data underlying the results are available as part of the article and no additional source data are required.

Extended data

Zenodo: Supplemental files for "Eating versus skipping breakfast has no discernible effect on obesity-related anthropometric outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis.". http://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.404139212.

This project contains the following extended data:

  • calculations.R (calculates individual effect sizes for each study)

  • metaanalysis with subgroup.R (reproduces the composite forest plot, leave-one-out plot, the summary table, and the subgroup analyses)

  • neumann2016.csv (contains the raw data from Neumann 2016 with authors’ corrections)

  • rho estimates for farshchi.R (uses data from Geliebter et al. to estimate within-condition pre-post correlations)

  • Subgroup analysis - methods and results.pdf (provides the methods, results, summary table, and forest plots for the subgroup interaction estimates between baseline habits and assigned breakfast conditions)

Reporting guidelines

Zenodo: PRISMA checklist for "Eating versus skipping breakfast has no discernible effect on obesity-related anthropometric outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis". http://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.404139212.

Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (CC-BY 4.0).

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 3
VERSION 3 PUBLISHED 24 Feb 2020
Comment
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Bohan Brown MM, Milanes JE, Allison DB and Brown AW. Eating versus skipping breakfast has no discernible effect on obesity-related anthropometric outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis [version 2; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2020, 9:140 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.22424.2)
NOTE: If applicable, it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 2
VERSION 2
PUBLISHED 15 Oct 2020
Revised
Views
29
Cite
Reviewer Report 03 Dec 2020
Nerys Astbury, Nuffield Department of Primary Care Health Sciences, NIHR Oxford Biomedical Research Centre, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 29
Although some changes have been made to this manuscript, I do not believe that some of the authors arguments are valid.

If the purpose of the review was to review the literature to determine whether breakfast has ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Astbury N. Reviewer Report For: Eating versus skipping breakfast has no discernible effect on obesity-related anthropometric outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis [version 2; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2020, 9:140 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.29951.r73123)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 30 Jul 2021
    Michelle M Brown, Department of Applied Health Science, Indiana University School of Public Health - Bloomington, Bloomington, 47405, USA
    30 Jul 2021
    Author Response
    We appreciate Dr. Astbury’s thoughtful comments. The comments reinforce that there are many details about recommendations to eat or skip breakfast that need to be considered beyond simplistic advice, including ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 30 Jul 2021
    Michelle M Brown, Department of Applied Health Science, Indiana University School of Public Health - Bloomington, Bloomington, 47405, USA
    30 Jul 2021
    Author Response
    We appreciate Dr. Astbury’s thoughtful comments. The comments reinforce that there are many details about recommendations to eat or skip breakfast that need to be considered beyond simplistic advice, including ... Continue reading
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 24 Feb 2020
Views
34
Cite
Reviewer Report 29 May 2020
Enhad A. Chowdhury, Department for Health, University of Bath, Bath, UK 
Approved
VIEWS 34
Overview

The work of Bohan Brown et al examines the impact of breakfast skipping versus consumption on obesity related anthropometric measures using meta-analytical methods. The authors have selected a focused question and I found the description of ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Chowdhury EA. Reviewer Report For: Eating versus skipping breakfast has no discernible effect on obesity-related anthropometric outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis [version 2; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2020, 9:140 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.24742.r62393)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 15 Oct 2020
    Michelle M Brown, Department of Applied Health Science, Indiana University School of Public Health - Bloomington, Bloomington, 47405, USA
    15 Oct 2020
    Author Response
    We thank Dr. Chowdhury for the feedback. We address the critiques below:

    Regarding Bonnet et al, we have now added the reference. It was published between our first submission ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 15 Oct 2020
    Michelle M Brown, Department of Applied Health Science, Indiana University School of Public Health - Bloomington, Bloomington, 47405, USA
    15 Oct 2020
    Author Response
    We thank Dr. Chowdhury for the feedback. We address the critiques below:

    Regarding Bonnet et al, we have now added the reference. It was published between our first submission ... Continue reading
Views
38
Cite
Reviewer Report 27 May 2020
Nerys Astbury, Nuffield Department of Primary Care Health Sciences, NIHR Oxford Biomedical Research Centre, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 38
This is a systematic review of randomised controlled trials comparing the consumption of breakfast with skipping breakfast and follows other similar SR on breakfast eating/skipping.

There has been a long held debate regarding the potential effect of ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Astbury N. Reviewer Report For: Eating versus skipping breakfast has no discernible effect on obesity-related anthropometric outcomes: a systematic review and meta-analysis [version 2; peer review: 1 approved, 1 approved with reservations]. F1000Research 2020, 9:140 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.24742.r62392)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 15 Oct 2020
    Michelle M Brown, Department of Applied Health Science, Indiana University School of Public Health - Bloomington, Bloomington, 47405, USA
    15 Oct 2020
    Author Response
    We thank Dr. Astbury for the feedback. We address the critiques below:

    We have added to our discussion to outline more completely some of the limitations and choices we ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 15 Oct 2020
    Michelle M Brown, Department of Applied Health Science, Indiana University School of Public Health - Bloomington, Bloomington, 47405, USA
    15 Oct 2020
    Author Response
    We thank Dr. Astbury for the feedback. We address the critiques below:

    We have added to our discussion to outline more completely some of the limitations and choices we ... Continue reading

Comments on this article Comments (0)

Version 3
VERSION 3 PUBLISHED 24 Feb 2020
Comment
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.