Keywords
explanation, prediction, inference, reasoning, development, story, preschoolers, India
This article is included in the Developmental Psychology and Cognition gateway.
This article is included in the Manipal Academy of Higher Education gateway.
Background: Considering the importance of exploring the development of reasoning skills during preschool period and the suitability of using a culturally linguistically relevant story-based approach for the same, the present research intended to profile the reasoning skills in typically developing Indian preschool children between 36 and 72 months using a story-based approach. The specific objectives were to determine the test-retest and inter-rater reliability of reasoning tasks within an existing story-based cognitive-communicative assessment tool and to use this tool to assess the reasoning skills of typically developing Indian preschool children.
Method: Reasoning tasks across explanation, prediction and inference domains were evaluated for its psychometric properties and administered to 63 typically developing Indian preschool children attending English medium schools in Mangalore. The preschoolers were equally divided into three age groups. The responses obtained across the age groups were analyzed quantitatively and qualitatively.
Results: The developed tasks were confirmed to have good psychometric properties like test-retest and inter-rater reliability. The age comparisons of reasoning abilities using one-way ANOVA suggested an increase in reasoning abilities with age during the preschool period. The qualitative analysis further suggested that with increasing age, the nature of reasoning changed from content-based reasoning to reasoning based on prior knowledge which was integrated with the story content.
Conclusion: The study describes reasoning skill development using a story-based task in Indian preschoolers. The study findings further provide clinical and educational implications to assess and foster reasoning abilities among preschoolers.
explanation, prediction, inference, reasoning, development, story, preschoolers, India
In this revised version, the discussion and future directions of the study has been revisited with the explanation for the test-retest reliability findings. Additional references quoted are added in the bibliography.
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Ramesh Kaipa
See the authors' detailed response to the review by Andreas Demetriou
Reasoning is a cognitive process of making inferences, drawing conclusions, or evaluating a proposed conclusion (Andrews, 2020). Reasoning skills enable individuals to understand and learn about the physical and social environment on a daily basis (Khemlani, 2018). The ability to appropriately reason across different life situations is essential for a successful professional and everyday life (Bronkhorst et al., 2020). It was long believed that reasoning skills do not develop until adolescence (Piaget, 1952). Although preschoolers are known as ‘little scientists’ with abundant curiosity and an innate drive to know the world (Alvarez & Booth, 2014; Piaget, 1952), reasoning skills are considered to be a higher-level cognitive skill that is beyond the reach of preschoolers (Whittaker & McMullen, 2014). Thinking abilities among preschoolers have often been described as “egocentric, pre-logical, affective, undifferentiated, pre-causal, personal, vague and unanalysed” (Piaget, 1952). However, some recent researchers indicate that the development of reasoning skills begins during the preschool years and continues through adolescence (Amsterlaw, 2006; Gopnik et al., 2004; Legare et al., 2010; Säre, Luik & Fisher, 2016a; Whittaker & McMullen, 2014).
During the preschool period, as children actively explore their environment through social interactions and physical manipulations, they achieve significant language and cognitive developmental milestones. This period marks crucial language developments such as the onset of sentence-level communication with simple, active declarative utterances, the use of functional and auxiliary words, pronouns, questions, tenses, conjunctions, possessives, initiation of conversations, event narrations, spurts of “why” questions, and “because” sentences (Conti-Ramsden & Durkin, 2012; Visser-Bochane et al., 2020). These language milestones facilitate expressing their thoughts as verbal reasoning (Socher et al., 2022). Likewise, cognitive abilities such as reasoning, processing speed, and cognitive flexibility significantly contribute to the comprehension and development of language in children (Willinger et al., 2019). Research postulates that questioning and exploratory talk with children trigger reasoning responses by encouraging them to explain their ideas, draw conclusions, and make predictions in general communication contexts. Thus, language and reasoning during developmental phases share a bidirectional relationship where development in one domain facilitates or enhances the other (Van de Sande et al., 2019; Willinger et al., 2019). This underscores the crucial role of understanding the development of reasoning skills during the preschool period, given its significant role in facilitating the development of other critical skills like language (Bauer & Booth, 2019; Nussipzhanova et al., 2018). Understanding reasoning skills during preschool years is also vital for several other critical decisions, such as preparing children to perceive and understand real-life scenarios, determining the right time for school entry, and laying the foundation for academic achievement and success (Niklas et al., 2018; Pasnak et al., 2015).
The existing literature suggests that reasoning abilities among preschoolers have been explored in domains like reasoning associated with improbable and less logical events like growing money on a tree, going back in time (Shtulman & Carey, 2007), counterfactual events (Muller et al., 2007), judgments on human abilities (Heyman et al., 2003), temporal ordering of events (McCormack & Hoerl, 2005), teleological functions (Kelemen et al., 2003), consistent and inconsistent events (Legare et al., 2010), pre-energy reasoning (Koliopoulos et al., 2009), analogies (Simms et al., 2018), and causal reasoning concerning scientific literacy associated with space, structures, tools, etc. (Bauer & Booth, 2019). However, the findings from these studies remain inconclusive in understanding the general reasoning development in preschoolers that could be fostered through education during the preschool period (Säre, Luik, & Fisher, 2016a). These studies seem to focus less on how children verbally reason in a general living context and how education could be the mediator to foster reasoning among preschoolers in such contexts. We could find one such recent initiative by Säre, Luik, and Fisher (2016a), wherein a valid and reliable tool ‘Younger Children Verbal Reasoning Test’ was developed to aid educators in assessing the general verbal reasoning of older preschoolers between five and six years of age. The tool utilized scenarios related to a theme on ‘bravery’ with illustrations and prompted children to judge how brave the characters are in the given scenario based on their actions. However, the authors of the work highlighted the psychometric properties of the developed tool and provided limited description regarding application of their tool in understanding general reasoning skills development in preschoolers.
The stimuli used to assess the reasoning abilities among preschoolers is a significant aspect to consider while discussing the development of general reasoning skills in this age group. In this context, Kendeou et al. (2019) put forth an integrated theoretical framework, inferential Language Comprehension (iLC), guiding and recommending the effective use of static or dynamic visual narratives (Stories) as a stimulus for evaluating reasoning skills among younger children. The study recommended that reasoning skills, such as inferencing opportunities, could be increased using static or dynamic visual narrative with questioning techniques in younger children who are non-readers. The story-based materials are reported to aid in better representation of general reasoning abilities in everyday communication contexts as they are more relatable to the natural learning context for the children (Killick & Boffey, 2012; O’Reilly et al., 2022). Stories also seem to provide an opportunity for children to provide explanations, form predictions, and make inferences that would help them to form logical, causally sequenced plots (Paris & Paris, 2003). Since a long, stories are known to constitute an inevitable part of childhood education, as children are intrinsically drawn to stories, and stories facilitate the development of thinking and learning in children (Paley, 2013). Preschool teachers highly appreciate the use of stories for fostering preschool child development and have recommended for its use as a stimuli for assessments (Prasanna et al., 2024b). Recent research by Dawes et al. (2019) advocates the potential and sensitivity of a story-based approach to obtain insight into preschoolers’ developmental changes for skills such as inferential comprehension. The application of story-based stimuli to assess reasoning skills during preschool years has ranged from asking questions in an open-ended format after a short story sequence with picture cards (Muller et al., 2007; Shtulman & Carey, 2007) or after short fragments of story scenarios depicted through illustrations and objects (Bauer & Booth, 2019; Heyman et al., 2003; McCormack & Hoerl, 2005; Simms et al., 2018). A recent review by O’Reilly et al. (2022) highlighted the effectiveness of stories in fostering critical thinking, such as reasoning and problem-solving among preschoolers, and advocated the utility of story-based approaches in the field of critical thinking for future researchers.
Cognitive assessments, in general, have been recommended to be culturally and linguistically relevant to the age and community of the participants for yielding accurate findings (Tanveer et al., 2022). Likewise, though stories offer significant potential in assessing reasoning abilities among preschool age children, it is important to note that stories should be culturally and linguistically relevant for getting a true insight into the developmental trends of reasoning abilities among preschoolers (Rao et al., 2021; Sternberg, 2004).
Some other crucial aspects while exploring general reasoning skills among preschoolers, besides the stimuli, are the ways reasoning based responses are elicited with the children and the different tasks employed to assess their abilities. An interventional research on facilitating reasoning among preschoolers used undirected, shared picture book narration within a peer-group setting (Reed et al., 2015). The study qualitatively analyzed the narration discourse by preschoolers while they freely generated stories from picture books and observed the emergence of reasoning categories such as explanation, prediction, and inference. The findings of this research informed that spontaneously elicited reasoning categories in pre-schoolers without guidance could better represent the reasoning developmental changes in the preschool period. Regarding the tasks for assessing reasoning among preschoolers, different studies have employed tasks based on explanation, predictions, and inferencing. In the explanation-based reasoning tasks, participants are expected to explain the reason behind an outcome (Legare et al., 2010) whereas, in the prediction-based reasoning tasks, participants are required to predict the outcome of an event assuming a cause (Bonawitz et al., 2010). The inference-based reasoning task expects the participants to form a conclusion about a connection between an antecedent and an outcome with rational justification (Gopnik et al., 2004). Similar reasoning tasks based on explanation, prediction, and inference were utilized in preschoolers to explore reasoning skills related to the physical causality of objects by Blank et al. (1981). It seems most promising to incorporate explanation, prediction, and inference based-reasoning tasks in a story-based approach while studying reasoning development during preschool years.
Thus, recognizing the critical role of reasoning in language development and the mutually facilitating nature of these skills, alongside the scarcity of studies on general reasoning development among preschoolers, the current research was initiated. Understanding the importance of using a culturally and linguistically sensitive tool and the suitability of a story-based approach, this research employed a story-based method to explore reasoning among preschoolers. Due to the lack of such culturally and linguistically relevant story-based tools in the Indian context, our previous work developed a tool for assessing cognitive-communication skills among Indian preschoolers based on a story and evaluated its content validity (Prasanna, et al., 2024b). The present research extends this work by utilizing the reasoning component (explanation, prediction, and inference-based reasoning tasks) of the developed tool to explore reasoning skills development among preschoolers. The aim of this research was to profile the reasoning skills of typically developing Indian preschool children aged 36 to 72 months using a story-based approach. The specific objectives were to determine the test-retest and inter-rater reliability of reasoning tasks within an existing story-based cognitive-communicative assessment tool and to use this tool to assess the reasoning skills of typically developing Indian preschool children. The study did not expect the developmental trends of reasoning abilities to be gender-specific, based on evidence from previous literature (Ardila et al., 2011).
The study followed a cross-sectional research design following STROBE guidelines and commenced after obtaining Institutional Ethical Clearance from Kasturba Medical College, Mangalore, Manipal Academy of Higher Education (IEC KMC MLR 02-19/51). The data collection was conducted between June 2021 to January 2022 in Mangalore, India. The present study serves as an evaluation phase of our previous work that developed a tool for assessing cognitive-communication skills among preschoolers in the Indian context based on a story (Prasanna et al., 2024b) and hence, the current study utilized the story-based cognitive-communication assessment tool developed in our previous work (Prasanna et al., 2024b) for exploring reasoning skills among preschoolers.
Typically developing children aged 36 to 72 months attending English medium schools in Mangalore were selected as participants. Written informed consent was obtained from school authorities, and parents of all the participants before their inclusion in the study. Initially, 76 children were screened for typical sensorimotor, cognitive, and language development using the Ten Questions Screen (TQS) (Durkin et al., 1995) and Assessment of Language Development (ALD) (Lakkanna et al., 2021). As TQS doesn’t have any cut-off scoring, children who screened negative across all the 10 items of the TQS, and those who exhibited age-adequate receptive and expressive language skills according to ALD were recruited. Since socioeconomic status highly correlates with the cognitive development of young children (Greenfield & Moorman, 2019), only those participants who belonged to middle socioeconomic status according to the modified Kuppuswamy socioeconomic scale (Saleem, 2019) were included in this study. Only those children who had attended nursery before joining preschool were included in the study to ensure homogeneity concerning exposure to literacy skills. The English language proficiency of the children was ascertained on Child Language Experience and Proficiency Questionnaire (LEAP-Q-Child version) and those who obtained seven or greater on a 10-point rating scale in the questionnaire were included in the study (Marian et al., 2007; Rochanavibhata et al., 2019). Thus, from a total of 76 children, 13 children were excluded from participation due to either history or complaint of deficits in sensory, motor, and/or language development, and only 63 participants were considered for the present study. The age and gender of the participants confirmed based on parental report, and the participants were equally divided into three groups according to age, i.e., Group I (Age range: 36–48 months, Mean age: 43 months, Standard deviation: 3.8 months, 10 Females and 11 Males), Group II (Age range: 49–60 months, Mean age: 54 months, Standard deviation: 3.8 months, 10 Females and 11 Males) and Group III (Age range: 61–72 months, Mean age: 65 months, Standard deviation: 3.2 months, 10 Females and 11 Males). All the shortlisted children had English as their second language, and among them, 89% were bilingual (L1-Kannada/Malayalam, L2-English), and 11% were multilingual (L1-Konkani/Hindi/Malayalam, L2-English, L3-Kannada). The sample size of the study was calculated using the formula with a 95% confidence level, 20% absolute error, and 80% power. The anticipated standard deviation of 0.789 was based on pilot study results.
Present research utilized the story-based cognitive-communication assessment tool developed in our previous work for Indian preschoolers (Prasanna et al., 2024b). The tool was developed following Delphi procedure in its conceptualization and construction, and the processes involved is illustrated in our previous work. The content validity index of greater than 0.78 for all the items of the tool revealed an excellent content validity for the developed tool as per the standard CVI criteria (Almanasreh et al., 2019; Polit & Beck, 2006).
The story of the tool was titled ‘A Day at Grandparents House’ (Prasanna et al., 2021b; Prasanna et al., 2024b) themed on a boy visiting grandparents’ house and the events happening at the house. The story was divided into three story sections with an equal distribution of 14 story elements per section with corresponding two pictures. Thereby the story contained a total of six pictures (Prasanna et al., 2021a; Prasanna et al., 2024b) presenting the story elements equally across auditory and auditory-visual modality (Table 1). The story was designed in English to maintain uniformity among participants from different native languages.
The reasoning tasks of the tool had been constructed based on the iLC framework proposed by Kendeou et al. (2019) which emphasized the utility of a static visual narrative and questioning technique for assessing reasoning abilities among preschoolers. The reasoning skills were designed across explanation, prediction, and inference-based reasoning tasks formulated for each of the sections of the story. Considering the potential of questioning as a strategy to assess reasoning among young children (Kendeou et al., 2019; Säre, Luik, & Tulviste, 2016b), the tasks were designed in a question based format. The questions regarding explanation and prediction based reasoning abilities were framed in an open-ended format (Eg: Why did Virat get scared? [explanation], What would have happened if grandpa had not locked the dog in the cage? [prediction]), whereas the questions for the inference based reasoning comprised of a closed-ended polar question (Yes/No type) to elicit the inference followed by an open-ended question to prompt the justification for the same (Eg: Did Virat love grandparents? [inference] followed by What made you feel so? [justification for the inference]).
Though the tool revealed the representation of the targeted construct of assessment with adequate content validity index in the previous work, other psychometric properties were not evaluated. Hence, in the present research, for further evaluation of the psychometric properties and to confirm the feasibility of the reasoning tasks, a pilot study among 15 typically developing children aged between 36 to 72 months was conducted. Since diverse answers could be possible for some of these reasoning tasks, especially among preschoolers, all the probable responses obtained during the pilot study were reviewed and discussed by the researchers to formulate an answer key. To obtain the test-retest reliability, the tasks were re-administered after two weeks among the 15 preschoolers (five from each age group), and to obtain the inter-rater reliability, two raters rated the data obtained from the 15 preschoolers. Since the tool developed is planned to be commercially released in the future, the optimum representation of the story text and reasoning tasks has been provided in Table 1 for the readers.
The data collection was carried out within the home premises, wherein the participants were seated in a well-lit, quiet room with only the researcher and the child present during the assessment. The evaluation was scheduled in the morning, considering the children’s active time according to the mother’s feedback. The researcher initially built rapport with the children and developed their interest in listening to the story through engaging in an active conversation about stories (For example: Do you like to listen to a story? Does your mother tell you stories? I am going to tell you a story. Would you like to know what happened in that story?). The researcher then instructed the child to carefully listen to the story and answer the questions, which was rewarded with reinforcement (stickers). The researcher also explained that if the child did not know the answer or wanted to think longer, they could freely say that. A practice trial was given to the child by listening to another short story (different from the story used in present assessment) and answering three reasoning-based questions based on the story to familiarise with the assessment procedure.
After the practice trial, the story was presented using a laptop and headphones. The reasoning tasks were performed after the presentation of each story section to ensure that excessive recall load did not dilute the findings. The order of the presentation of the questions in each section was randomized for each participant using a random number generator. Children were encouraged to respond verbally to all the questions and were rewarded with reinforcement (stickers). The assessment took around 20 minutes per participant, including a one-minute rest after each story section and task.
The content validation of the tool was estimated in the previous work (Prasanna et al., 2024b), hence in the present research further evaluation interms of the test-retest reliability and inter-rater reliability was established using the Intra class correlation coefficient (ICC) analysis at 95% confidence interval with absolute agreement using two-way mixed model.
The responses obtained from the assessment of reasoning skills using the three tasks were analyzed quantitatively and qualitatively. For the quantitative analysis, the responses across the explanation, prediction, and justification components of the inference tasks were scored based on their appropriateness. The appropriateness of the answers was determined based on the answer key prepared by the researchers, considering logic and rationality. Children received a score of ‘one’ for the appropriate reasons and ‘zero’ for the inappropriate or no reasons for all the tasks. For example, for an explanation based question, ‘Why did Virat get scared?’, if the child’s response was ‘because he saw the dog’ or ‘because the dog would bite,’ it was interpreted as appropriate and received a score of ‘one.’ Whereas if the child’s response to the same question was unrelated (such as ‘because he saw a ghost’ or ‘don’t’ know’), then such answers were considered inappropriate and scored ‘zero’ accordingly. The closed-end polar questions (Yes/No type) of the inference based reasoning task were scored as ‘one’ for the correct answers and ‘zero’ for the incorrect answers. A child could thus obtain a maximum score of ‘six’ for each reasoning task. To prevent bias in the scoring, two independent researchers who were blind to the participants’ demographic details scored the responses, and any disagreements were resolved by reaching a consensus through discussion with a third independent researcher. The scored data were subjected to one-way ANOVA with post hoc Tukey test using SPSS V.25 software to investigate the quantitative changes in reasoning skills with age during the preschool period. Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) analysis was also carried out to determine the cut-off values distinguishing the significant quantitative changes in reasoning skills revealed during ANOVA analysis. Gender wise analysis of the task scores was carried out using Independent t test.
The data was further analyzed qualitatively, exploring the manner in which children reason with increase in age to get an insight into the typical pattern of reasoning during the preschool years. Such an analysis was expected to serve as a guide for stimulating typical reasoning development in clinical and educational settings. A general inductive approach (Thomas, 2006), a systematic set of procedures for analyzing qualitative data that produce reliable and valid findings, was followed for qualitative analysis of the reasoning responses. Two independent researchers coded and categorized the responses for explanation, prediction, and justification of the inference tasks. The codes and categories obtained from the two researchers were compared and merged into a combined set to establish the extent of overlap. The agreement between the two coders was set at 95%, and disagreements were resolved through discussion with a third independent researcher. Based on this process and the consensus among the researchers, the responses of the participants were qualitatively categorized into ‘Appropriate reasoning with story content,’ ‘Appropriate reasoning integrating story content with prior knowledge,’ ‘Inappropriate reasoning with improper story content usage,’ and ‘Inappropriate reasoning with context associated with prior knowledge but improper to the story content’ and ‘No reason.’ An example of the qualitative analysis of one such response from the first section of the story is provided in Table 2. For more details of the associated data see underlying data (Prasanna et al., 2023a).
The psychometric properties of the tasks were determined using test-retest reliability and inter-rater reliability measures. The test-retest reliability measures revealed an ICC of 0.66 indicating moderate test-retest reliability and inter-rater reliability measures revealed an ICC of 0.91 indicating excellent inter-rater reliability at 95% confidence interval with absolute agreement using two-way mixed model.
The story-based tasks were then used to assess the reasoning abilities of 63 typically developing Indian preschool children. The responses obtained from the participants across the explanation, prediction, and inference based reasoning tasks were quantitatively and qualitatively analyzed. The mean and standard deviation of the total reasoning scores and the percentage of categories of responses for the explanation, prediction, and inference based reasoning tasks across the age groups are illustrated in Figure 1(a,b), Figure 2(a,b), and Figure 3(a-c) respectively.
A comparison of the mean scores of the explanation based reasoning tasks revealed that the explanation based reasoning scores increased with age, F (2,60)=17.91, p<0.001, irrespective of the gender (t[61]=0.401,p=0.690). The post hoc pairwise comparison showed that the older groups (Groups II and III) performed significantly better (p<0.01) than the younger group (Group I); however, there was no statistically significant difference between the older groups (p=0.08). Receiver Operating Characteristic (ROC) analysis revealed a cut-off score of 2.5 discriminating between the younger (Group I) and older groups (Groups II and III) for explanation-based reasoning performance with 81% sensitivity, 62% specificity, and 0.8 Area under the curve (AUC) that is statistically significant (p<0.05).
The qualitative analysis using the percentage of categories of reasoning responses showed that with the increase in age, most children reasoned appropriately, integrating story content with prior knowledge. The codes identified under appropriate reasoning categories and their frequency across the age groups under the explanation task are presented in Table 3.
The comparison of the mean scores for the prediction based reasoning task also showed a significant increase with age F (2,60)=26.58, p<0.001, irrespective of the gender t (61)=0.564, p=0.575. Post hoc pairwise comparison revealed a significant difference in prediction scores between all three age groups (p<0.01). The ROC analysis revealed a cut-off prediction score of 4.5 discriminating between Group I and Group II with 52% sensitivity, 90% specificity, and 0.8 statistically significant AUC (p<0.001), and a cut-off prediction score of 5.5 discriminating Group II from Group III with 52% sensitivity, 86% specificity, and 0.7 AUC, that is statistically significant (p<0.05).
The qualitative analysis showed that similar to the explanation task, most children reasoned integrating story content with prior knowledge as their age increased. The codes identified under appropriate reasoning categories and their frequency across the age groups under the prediction task are presented in Table 4.
For the inference-based reasoning tasks, comparison of the mean score revealed that the inference scores remained almost similar across the age groups, F (2,60)=0.206, p=0.82, irrespective of the gender (t [61]=0.167, p=0.868). However, the scores pertaining to the justification for inference were found to increase with age, F (2,60)=6.04, p<0.05, without gender as a factor (t [61]=1.767, p=0.082). Post hoc pairwise comparison revealed significant differences in the inference justification performance between older groups (Groups II and III) and younger group (Group I) (p<0.05). There was no statistically significant difference between the older groups (Group II and III) (p=0.99). ROC analysis determined the cut-off score as 0.5, discriminating between the younger (Group I) and older groups (Groups II and III) for inference justification scores with 62% sensitivity, 86% specificity, and 0.7 statistically significant AUC (p<0.05).
Qualitative analysis showed that most of the responses belong to the ‘No reason’ category across the age groups and a smaller percentage of responses under ‘appropriate reasoning’ categories. Among the ‘appropriate categories,’ majority of the preschoolers reasoned under the category of ‘appropriate reasoning with story content.’ The codes identified under the smaller percentage of appropriate reasoning categories and their frequency across the age groups under the justification of inference task are presented in Table 5.
The evaluation of the psychometric properties of the story-based reasoning tasks revealed excellent inter-rater reliability and moderate test-retest reliability. The moderate test-retest reliability of executive function-based cognitive tasks during the preschool period, as observed in this study, is consistent with existing literature, which reports similar reliability ranges (0.52–0.66) for this stage of development (Espy & Cwik, 2004; Isquith et al., 2005; Na et al., 2024; Willoughby & Blair, 2011). Studies have attributed moderate test-retest reliabilities in preschoolers to several key factors. First, the rapid developmental changes typical of this age group (Na et al., 2024) can significantly impact performance consistency. Additionally, preschoolers' fluctuating effort and concentration contribute to variations in task engagement (Espy & Cwik, 2004). Moreover, cognitive constructs such as executive functions are less differentiated in early childhood, which leads to higher reliability when assessed as broad constructs, rather than in specific domains like reasoning, as seen in the present study (Willoughby & Blair, 2011). Nevertheless, the small sample size in the present study, along with factors such as the interval between assessments and the challenge of maintaining a consistent level of attention among preschoolers during both sessions, suggests potential for further refinement. Optimizing these elements may help achieve higher test-retest reliability for the reasoning tasks.
The tool utilized that consisted of a story and reasoning tasks in a question format aligned with the theoretical framework proposed for exploring reasoning skills among preschoolers (Kendeou et al., 2019). A recent survey among teachers in Indian context also highlights the potential of stories among preschoolers for assessments (Prasanna et al., 2024a). The use of story as the assessment material is also consistent with the other research that supports the efficacy of story-based approaches among preschoolers (Dawes et al., 2019; O’Reilly et al., 2022). Besides adding culturally and linguistically relevant data on reasoning skills to the existing literature in this domain among preschoolers (Muller et al., 2007; Shtulman & Carey, 2007), the current study has offered additional depth by exploring the development of reasoning skills across explanation, prediction and inference domains using a single story. Moreover, the present format of these tasks, wherein domains of the reasoning abilities could be assessed on spontaneously emerged output of typically developing preschoolers, is aligned with the earlier perspectives in this regard (Reed et al., 2015). Further, the components of the story of the tool, such as the characters, sequences, objects, dress, pictures, vocabulary, and task formats, were designed to reflect the real-life Indian context, which could offer appropriate familiarity and suitability to Indian preschoolers and aid in accurate representation of their abilities (Rao et al., 2021; Sternberg, 2004).
The story-based tasks assessed the reasoning abilities of typically developing Indian preschool children. The comparison of mean reasoning scores across the age groups in the explanation, prediction, and justification of inference task revealed that the preschooler’s reasoning skills improved significantly with age. In most tasks (explanation and justification of inference), a significant change in performance was observed from Group I (36–48 months) to Group II (49–60 months) and III (61–72 months) than between Group II (49–60 months) and III (61–72 months), indicating the rapid development of reasoning skills during the initial phase of the preschool period. The results agree with the previous literature that younger preschoolers show a significant improvement in reasoning skills with age than older preschoolers (Hong et al., 2005). The results also imitate the age bands of thinking differences (Symbolic function substage: 2–4 years, Intuitive thought substage: 4 to 7 years) in Piaget’s preoperational stage of cognitive development (Piaget, 1952). However, the results obtained in the prediction task showed evidence for age-wise developmental changes than in age bands, with significant improvement in performance with age in years. The improved performance in reasoning skills with age across the tasks was not surprising as the preschool period is known to be critical for most of the developmental domains, including executive function skills (Garon et al., 2008).
Contrary to the expectation, the age-related difference in reasoning performance was not found in the inference task. More specifically, the findings from the inference task suggested that Group I (36–48 months) children performed like the Group II (49–60 months) and III (61–72 months) children. These results indicate that children as young as 36–48 months of age (Group I) can make inferences. Previous literature supports the findings attesting that preschoolers make logical inferences easily and earlier in development (Moshman, 2004). However, we could not exclude the fact that the inference task’s yes/no question nature might also have influenced the extent of this lack of age-related differences. In yes/no questions, researchers opine that younger preschoolers might show a yes/no bias (all yes responses, i.e., yes bias and vice versa) due to social pressure or difficulty inhibiting yes responses (Okanda & Itakura, 2010). Therefore, there is a chance that the younger participants in this study might have also undergone such yes/no bias in the inference task influencing the lack of age-related differences. Hence, it indicates that, though yes/no questions have been recommended for reasoning skills assessment (Säre et al., 2017), we need to be cautious regarding the chance of yes/no bias in younger preschoolers. However, despite the probability of yes/no bias in this study, the preschooler’s early inferential ability observed in the previous literature adds substantial weight to the conclusion of inferential ability in younger preschoolers.
Another interesting finding comparing mean reasoning scores was that the preschoolers performed poorly on the justification of inference task than on the other tasks. Though preschoolers could make inferences, the findings contend that they find it difficult to justify their inferences. This difficulty could be because the ability to justify the inferences is a higher quality of thinking requiring metacognition (Moshman, 2004). Though metacognition was not explicitly studied in this research, previous literature findings indicate that children require metacognitive awareness to justify their inferences (Whittaker & McMullen, 2014), which is beyond the preschool age in development (Veenman et al., 2006), lend support for the poorer performance in the current study. However, it is also noteworthy that a smaller percentage of older preschoolers (Group II, 49–60 months and Group III, 61–72 months) could provide appropriate responses (20%) in the justification of inference tasks using story content. Though it could be due to individual differences, it might also indicate the traces of metalogical development for reasoning from 4 years of age using the linguistic content, suggesting the scope of improvement in justification if provided with support.
On qualitative analysis, results from the explanation and prediction task revealed that among the appropriate reasons made by the preschoolers, younger preschoolers (Group I, 36–48 months) predominantly reason under the category of ‘reasoning with story content,’ and the older preschoolers (Group II, 49–60 months and Group III, 61–72 months) reason in the category of ‘reasoning integrating story content with prior knowledge.’ The results indicate that while younger preschoolers reason with reference to the inference based on the story content premise, older preschoolers undergo a higher thinking process of coordinating the logical inferences from the story and prior knowledge to conclude and reason. The results support the previous literature findings that preschoolers integrate inferences from linguistic context (Florit et al., 2011) and access prior knowledge while reasoning (Gopnik et al., 2004). The findings also align with the comprehension development process, which activates and integrates linguistic knowledge with background knowledge to connect the story contents and form a mental representation to comprehend (Kendeou et al., 2019).
Another interesting finding in qualitative analysis was with respect to the justification of the inference task. The results from the justification of inference task revealed that preschoolers depend on the source of knowledge from the linguistic contents to justify their inference. We have observed that older preschoolers tried to justify inference with respect to the source of knowledge from the story contents rather than the prior knowledge premise. It was also noted that most preschoolers provided empty reasons such as ‘I know’ for justification questions, claiming they had always known the inference. The findings are supported by the previous literature (Kuhn et al., 2004; Taylor et al., 1994) that children tend to give such responses when unsure or unaware of the answer. This could be because the metacognitive awareness of reasoning is in the emerging stage during the preschool period (Veenman et al., 2006). Based on the pattern of development observed in other tasks of reasoning, it could be expected that with increase in age over the preschool period, with the development of metacognitive awareness of reasoning, children might justify their inference majorly by integrating the story content with their prior knowledge and own interpretation, thereby following the similar pattern of reasoning development.
All the developmental changes in reasoning obtained in this study could be attributed to the biological maturational change of the brain (Andrews, 2020). While the reasoning skills are linked with specific brain regions such as the rostro lateral prefrontal cortex, inferior parietal lobe, cingulate cortex, corpus callosum among adolescents and adults (Feng et al., 2020; Wendelken et al., 2016; Willbrand et al., 2023; Wright et al., 2008), in the case of younger children, as the brain structures are less mature, rather than a specific region, the improvement in processing speed with the increased neural connection formations has been associated (Andrews, 2020; Kail et al., 2016; Tooley et al., 2022; Wendelken et al., 2016, 2017). Literature indicates that the improvement in processing speed with structural connectivity changes such as neural network formations, global white matter integrity, changes in the brain volume, and reduction in cortical thickness in the fronto parietal regions correspond with reasoning development (Baron & Leonberger, 2012; Botdorf & Riggins, 2018; Bruchhage et al., 2020; Chen et al., 2017; Ferrer et al., 2014; Wendelken et al., 2016). The preschool period which blossoms through biological maturation process of neural network formations (Brown & Jernigan, 2012), thereby could have contributed to the improvement in reasoning skills observed in the current research. These biological maturational changes could also have been facilitated by the everyday interactional context (Niklas et al., 2018; Säre, Luik, & Tulviste, 2016b), working memory (Andrews, 2020), language (Richland & Burchinal, 2013) and theory of mind development (Taggart et al., 2005; Whittaker & McMullen, 2014). Consistent with existing literature gender wise differences were not found in all the reasoning tasks (Ardila et al., 2011). The study’s findings need to be cautiously generalized, as reasoning responses are dynamic.
In conclusion, the study describes the general reasoning skill development in typically developing Indian preschool children between 36 to 72 months of age using story-based tasks. The study utilized a valid and reliable story-based task for the purpose, and the findings concluded that reasoning skills increase with age during preschool in a pattern of moving from reasoning based on story content to reasoning integrating the story content with prior knowledge. The study findings provide clinical and educational implications with insights on general reasoning development in preschoolers and the utility of age-appropriate story-based explanation, prediction, and inference question tasks to assess and foster reasoning responses in preschoolers. The findings also guide educators to scaffold reasoning in preschoolers, and the tool aid in early identification of reasoning deficits and guide planning intervention for Indian preschoolers with reasoning deficits. Though the study findings provide insight into general reasoning development in preschoolers, the sampler size was smaller and the role of contributing factors such as working memory, theory of mind, language, bilingualism/multilingualism, and social interaction on reasoning was not explored, and it remains a limitation of the present study. Therefore, future studies considering such factors could provide an in-depth understanding of reasoning development to assess and foster reasoning during preschool years. Additionally, the test-retest reliability of the given tasks could be further investigated by optimizing the sample size, time interval, and the attentiveness of participants between the two assessments.
Figshare: ‘An Insight into developmental changes in reasoning skills among Indian Preschoolers - A Cross-Sectional Study using a story-based approach’, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.22140617.v1 (Prasanna et al., 2023a).
This project contains the following underlying data:
Figshare: STROBE checklist for ‘An Insight into developmental changes in reasoning skills among Indian Preschoolers - A Cross-Sectional Study using a story-based approach’, https://doi.org/10.6084/m9.figshare.22140689.v2 (Prasanna et al., 2023b).
Data are available under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license (CC-BY 4.0).
Views | Downloads | |
---|---|---|
F1000Research | - | - |
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
|
- | - |
Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?
Yes
Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?
Yes
Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?
Yes
If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?
Yes
Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?
Yes
Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?
Yes
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: Early childhood Care and Education, Spatial intelligence
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: Speech and Language Therapy
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: Speech and Language Therapy
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: Cognitive development and individual differences in intelligence. Integration of cognitive developmental and psychometric theory.
Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?
Yes
Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?
Partly
Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?
Yes
If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?
Yes
Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?
Yes
Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?
No
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: Cognitive development and individual differences in intelligence. Integration of cognitive developmental and psychometric theory.
Is the work clearly and accurately presented and does it cite the current literature?
Partly
Is the study design appropriate and is the work technically sound?
Yes
Are sufficient details of methods and analysis provided to allow replication by others?
Yes
If applicable, is the statistical analysis and its interpretation appropriate?
Yes
Are all the source data underlying the results available to ensure full reproducibility?
Yes
Are the conclusions drawn adequately supported by the results?
Partly
Competing Interests: No competing interests were disclosed.
Reviewer Expertise: Speech and Language Therapy
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Invited Reviewers | |||
---|---|---|---|
1 | 2 | 3 | |
Version 3 (revision) 11 Oct 24 |
read | read | |
Version 2 (revision) 09 Jul 24 |
read | read | |
Version 1 26 Apr 23 |
read | read |
Provide sufficient details of any financial or non-financial competing interests to enable users to assess whether your comments might lead a reasonable person to question your impartiality. Consider the following examples, but note that this is not an exhaustive list:
Sign up for content alerts and receive a weekly or monthly email with all newly published articles
Already registered? Sign in
The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.
You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.
You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.
To sign in, please click here.
If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.
If your email address is registered with us, we will email you instructions to reset your password.
If you think you should have received this email but it has not arrived, please check your spam filters and/or contact for further assistance.
Comments on this article Comments (0)