ALL Metrics
-
Views
-
Downloads
Get PDF
Get XML
Cite
Export
Track
Research Article

Does evidence support the high expectations placed in precision medicine? A bibliographic review

[version 1; peer review: 2 approved with reservations, 1 not approved]
PUBLISHED 09 Jan 2018
Author details Author details
OPEN PEER REVIEW
REVIEWER STATUS

Abstract

Background: Precision medicine is the Holy Grail of interventions that are tailored to a patient’s individual characteristics. However, the conventional design of randomized trials assumes that each individual benefits by the same amount.
Methods: We reviewed parallel trials with quantitative outcomes published in 2004, 2007, 2010 and 2013. We collected baseline and final standard deviations of the main outcome. We assessed homoscedasticity by comparing the outcome variability between treated and control arms.
Results: The review provided 208 articles with enough information to conduct the analysis. At the end of the study, 113 (54%, 95% CI 47 to 61%) papers find less variability in the treated arm. The adjusted point estimate of the mean ratio (treated to control group) of the outcome variances is 0.89 (95% CI 0.81 to 0.97).
Conclusions: Some variance inflation was observed in just 1 out of 6 interventions, suggesting the need for further eligibility criteria to tailor precision medicine. Surprisingly, the variance was more often smaller in the intervention group, suggesting, if anything, a reduced role for precision medicine.  Homoscedasticity is a useful tool for assessing whether or not the premise of constant effect is reasonable.

Keywords

Constant Effect, Precision medicine, Homoscedasticity, Clinical Trial, Variability, Standard deviation, Review

Introduction

The idea behind precision medicine is to develop prevention and treatment strategies that take into account individual characteristics. With this strong endorsement “The prospect of applying this concept broadly has been dramatically improved by recent developments in large-scale biologic databases (such as the human genome sequence), powerful methods for characterizing patients (such as proteomics, metabolomics, genomics, diverse cellular assays, and mobile health technology), and computational tools for analyzing large sets of data.”, US President Obama launched the Precision Medicine initiative in 2015 to capitalize on these developments1,2. However, we are not convinced that this is a sensible idea.

Variability of a clinical trial outcome measure should interest researchers because it conveys important information about whether there is a need for precision medicine. Does variance come only from unpredictable and ineluctable sources of patient variability? Or should it also be attributed to a different treatment effect that requires more precise prescription rules35? Researchers assess treatment effect modifications (“interactions”) among subgroups based on relevant variables. The main problem with that methodology is that, by the usual standards of classical phase III trial, the stratification factors must be known in advance and be measurable. This in turn implies that when new variables are discovered and introduced into the causal path, new clinical trials are needed. Fortunately, one observable consequence of a constant effect is that the treatment will not affect variability, and therefore the outcome variances in both arms should be equal (“homoscedasticity”). If this homoscedasticity holds, there is no need to repeat the clinical trial once a new possible effect modifier becomes measurable.

Nevertheless, the fundamental problem of causal inference is that for each patient in a parallel group trial, we can know the response for only one of the interventions. That is, we observe their response to either the new Treatment or to the Control, but not both. By experimentally controlling unknown confounders through randomization, a clinical trial may estimate the averaged causal effect. In order to translate this population estimate into effects for individual patients, additional assumptions are needed. We try to elucidate whether the comparison of observed variances may shed some light on the non-observable individual treatment effect. See examples and references that illustrate in their interpretation in Figure 1615.

c35fa272-0759-480c-8618-794dfb9254ab_figure1.gif

Figure 1. Scenarios representing fictional trials with 8 participants with Systolic Blood Pressure as the primary endpoint.

Because of the random allocation to one of two treatment arms, we will only observe one of the two potential outcomes for each patient: either under T or under C. Fully saturated colors represent observed Systolic Blood Pressure (SBP) values, and transparent squares represent missing potential values. The line slope indicates the individual non-observable effect for each patient. Densities are the potential distributions of the outcome in each group: As both random samples come from the same target population, the average causal effect is estimable without bias. Panel A shows the potential outcome values that we could obtain if there were not any treatment effect; as the intervention has no effect at all, both groups have the same distribution (i.e., mean and variance). Panel B shows the scenario of a constant effect, meaning that the intervention lowers the SBP by a single value in every patient, implying the same variability in both arms. For instance, the study from Duran-Cantolla et al.6 compared the 24-hour systolic blood pressure among 340 patients randomized to either Continuous Positive Airway Pressure (CPAP) or sham–CPAP, and it showed a greater decrease of 2.1 mmHg (95% CI from 0.4 to 3.7) in the intervention group compared to the control group. Furthermore, baseline standard deviations (SDs) were 12 and 11; and final SDs were 13 for both groups. Therefore, their results fully agree with the trial design’s assumption of a constant effect (scenario B) and nothing contradicts the inference that each patient exhibits a constant reduction of 2.1mmHg, although the uncertainty of random allocation makes the results compatible with a constant effect that lies anywhere between 0.4 and 3.7. Panel C represents a situation with 2 different effects in 2 subpopulations (“treatment by subgroup interaction”). Although the effects are identical within them, the observable distribution in the treated arm would have higher variability. Here, we need to find finer eligibility criteria for classifying patients in those subpopulations so that a constant effect could be assumed again. In Panel D, the treatment has a variable effect in each patient, resulting also in greater variability within the treated arm but without any subgroup sharing a common effect, and results are poorly predictive about the effects on future patients. In the study by Kojima et al.7, the primary outcome measure was the 3-hour postprandial area under the curve of apolipoprotein B48, with outcome SDs being 0.78 and 0.16 in the treated and reference arms, respectively, thus showing a variance ratio of 23.77. This is compatible with different treatment effects that could need further refinements through precision medicine, since a greater variance in the treated arm indicates that “the interpretation of the main treatment effect is controversial”8. In that case, guidelines for treating new patients should be based either on additional eligibility criteria (“precision medicine”, panel C) or on n-of-1 trials (“individualized medicine”, panel D)913. This “treatment by patient interaction” was already highlighted by W. S. Gosset in the data of his 1908 paper proposing the Student t-distribution14. Alternatively, interactions can result in smaller variances in the treated arm. Panel E shows a different effect in 2 subgroups, but the variability is now reduced indicating that the best solution would be to identify the subpopulations in order to refine the selection criteria. In Panel F, the treatment has a stabilizing effect, with higher blood pressure falling more in severe patients, thus resulting in lower variability in the treatment arm. In the study from Kim et al.15, the primary endpoint was the PTSD Checklist–Civilian version (PCL-C). This scale is based on the sum of 17 Likert symptoms, ranging from 17 (perfect health) to 85 (worst clinical situation). At the end of the trial, the respective outcome SDs were 16 and 3 for the control and treated arms, meaning that variance was reduced around 28 times. This situation can correspond to scenarios E or F and merit much more statistical considerations, which is beyond the scope of this paper.

The assumption that the average effect equals the single unit effect underlies the rationale behind the usual sample size calculation, where only a single effect is specified. As an example, the 10 clinical trials published in the Trials Journal in October 2017 (see Supplementary File 1 : Table S1) were designed under this scenario of a fixed, constant or unique effect in the sample size calculation.

Our objectives were, first, to compare the variability of the main outcome between different arms in clinical trials published in medical journals and, second, to provide a first, rough estimate of the proportion of studies that could potentially benefit from precision medicine. As sensitivity analysis, we explore the changes in the experimental arm’s variability over time (from baseline to the end of the study). We also fit a random effect model to the outcome variance ratio in order to isolate studies with a variance ratio outside their expected random variability values (heterogeneity).

Methods

Population

Our target population was parallel randomized clinical trials with quantitative outcomes. Trials needed to provide enough information to assess two homoscedasticity assumptions in the primary endpoint: between arms at trial end; and baseline to outcome over time in the treated arm. Therefore, baseline and final SDs for the main outcome were necessary or, failing that, at least one measure that would allow us to calculate them (variances, standard errors or mean confidence intervals).

Data collection

Articles on parallel clinical trials from the years 2004, 2007, 2010 and 2013 were selected from the Medline database with the following criteria: “AB (clinical trial* AND random*) AND AB (change OR evolution OR (difference AND baseline)” [The word “difference” was paired with “baseline” because the initial purpose of the data collection, subsequently modified, was to estimate the correlation between baseline and final measurements]. The rationale behind the election of these years was to have a global view of the behavior of the studies over a whole decade. For the years 2004 and 2007, we selected all papers that met the inclusion criteria; while for the years 2010 and 2013, as we obtained a greater number of articles retrieved from the search (478 and 653, respectively), we chose a random sample of 300 papers (Section II in Supplementary File 1).

Data were collected by two different researchers (NM, MkV) in two phases: 2004/2007 and 2010/2013. Later, two statisticians (JC, MtV) verified the correctness of the data and to make them accessible to readers through a shiny application and through the Figshare repository16.

Variables

Collected variables were: baseline and outcome SDs; experimental and reference interventions; sample size in each group; medical field according to Web of Science (WOS) classification; main outcome; patient’s disease; kind of disease (chronic or acute); outcome type (measured or scored); intervention type (pharmacological or not); and whether or not the main effect was significant.

For studies with more than one quantitative outcome, primary endpoint was determined according to the following hierarchical criteria: (1) objective or hypothesis; (2) sample size determination; (3) main statistical method; or (4) first quantitative variable reported in results.

In the same way, the choice of the "experimental" arm was determined depending on the role in the following sections of the article: (1) objective or hypothesis; (2) sample size determination; (3) rationale in the introduction or (4) first comparison reported in results (in the case of more than two arms)

Statistical analysis

We assessed homoscedasticity between treatments and over time. Our main analysis compared, for the former, the outcome variability between Treated (T) and Control (C) arms at the trial end. For the latter, we compared the variability between Outcome (O) and its Baseline (B) value for the treated arm.

To distinguish between random variability and heterogeneity, we fitted a random mixed effects model using the logarithm of the variance ratio at the end of the trial as response with the study as random effect and the logarithm of the variance ratio at baseline as fixed effect17. An analogous model was employed to assess the homoscedasticity over time, as such a model allows the separation of random allocation variability from additional heterogeneity. To obtain a reference in the absence of treatment effect, we first modeled the baseline variance ratio as a response that is expected to have heterogeneity equal to 0 due to randomization – so long as no methodological impurities are present.(e.g., consider the outcomes obtained 1 month after the start of treatment as the baseline values). This reference model allows us to know the proportion of studies in the previous models that could have additional heterogeneity which cannot be explained by the variability among studies (sections III, V and VI in Supplementary File 1).

Funnel plots, centered at zero, of the measurement of interest as a function of its standard errors are reported in order to investigate asymmetries.

As sensitivity analyses, we assessed homoscedasticity in each single study: (a) between outcomes on both arms with F-test for independent samples; and (b) between baseline and outcome in the treated arm with a specific test for paired samples18 when the variance of the paired difference was available. All tests were two-sided (α=5%).

Several subgroup analyses were carried out according to the statistical significance of the main effect of the study and to the different types of outcomes and interventions.

All analyses were performed with the R statistical package version 3.2.5. (The R code for the main analysis is available from https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.113360919)

Results

Population

A total of 1214 articles were retrieved from the search. Of those papers, 542 belong to the target population and 208 (38.4%) contained enough information to conduct the analysis (Figure 2).

c35fa272-0759-480c-8618-794dfb9254ab_figure2.gif

Figure 2. Flow-chart of the articles in the study.

Percentages represent the quantity of papers in the target population. The number of articles for each year (2004/2007/2010/2013) is specified in the second line of each box (separated by slashes). $300 papers were randomly selected for years 2010 and 2013. *Four papers were excluded because the variance of the change over time was inconsistent with both the baseline and final variances, which would lead to impossible absolute correlation estimates greater than 1.

Mainly, the selected studies were non-pharmacological (122, 58.6%), referred to chronic conditions (101, 57.4%), had an outcome measure with units (132, 63.8%) instead of a constructed scale, and this outcome were measured rather than assessed (125, 60.1%). Regarding the primary objective of each trial, the authors found statistically significant differences between the arms in 83 (39.9%) studies. Following the WOS criteria, 203 articles (97.6%) belonged to at least one medical field. The main areas of study were: General & Internal Medicine (n=31, 14.9%), Nutrition & Dietetics (21, 10.1%), Endocrinology & Metabolism (19, 9.1%), and Cardiovascular System & Cardiology (16, 7.7%).

Homoscedasticity

There is a high average concordance between variances in the treatment and control arm, but with evidence of a smaller variability in the treated arm. At the end of the study, 113/208 (54%, 95% CI, 47 to 61%) papers showed less variability in the treated arm (Supplementary File 1 : Figure S1). Among the treated arms, 111/208 (53%, 95% CI, 46 to 60%) had less or equal variability at the end of follow-up than at the beginning (Supplementary File 1 : Figure S2).

We found statistically significant differences (at 5%) between outcome variances in 41 out of 208 (19.7%) studies: 7.2% were in favor of greater variance in the treated arm, and 12.5% in the opposite direction. A greater proportion was obtained from the comparisons over time of 95 treated arms: 16.8% had significantly greater variability at the end of the study and 23.2% at the beginning (Table 1).

Table 1. Variance comparison.

Alternative possible methods to estimate the number and percentage of studies with different variances on comparisons between arms and over-time. Limits for declaring different variances come from different statistical methods; either masked specified statistical tests (F for independent outcomes or Sachs’ test18 for related samples); or sensitivity analysis about the number of studies that have to be deleted from the random mixed model in order to achieve a negligible heterogeneity (see Methods for details). ¥ Only performed in studies reporting enough information to obtain the variability of the change from baseline to outcome, for example because they provide the correlation.

Comparing variancesNMethodAfter treatment, variability is…
Increased
n (%)
Decreased
n (%)
Not changed
n (%)
Outcome between
treatment arms
208F test15 (7.2%)26 (12.5%)167 (80.3%)
Random model11 (5.3%)19 (9.1%)178 (85.6%)
Outcome versus
baseline in treated arm
95¥Paired test16 (16.8%)22 (23.2%)57 (60.0%)
Random model13 (13.7%)19 (20.0%)63 (66.3%)

Regarding the comparison between arms, the adjusted point estimate of the ratio (Treated to Control group) of the outcome variances is 0.89 (95% CI 0.81 to 0.97), indicating that treatments tend to reduce the variability of the patient's response by about 11% on average. The comparison over time provides a similar result: the average variability at the end of the study is 14% lower than that at the beginning (Supplementary File 1 : Table S4).

According to the random model, the baseline heterogeneity was 0.31; this is a very high value which can only be explained by methodological flaws similar to those presented by Carlisle20. Fortunately, the exclusion of the four most extreme papers reduced it to 0.07; one of them was the study by Hsieh et al.21 whose “baseline” values were obtained 1 month after the treatment started. When we modeled the outcome instead of the baseline variances as the response, heterogeneity was approximately doubled. We found 30 studies that compromised homoscedasticity (11 with higher variance in the treated arm and 19 with lower, Table 1). Figure 3 shows the funnel plots for both between-arm and over-time comparisons.

c35fa272-0759-480c-8618-794dfb9254ab_figure3.gif

Figure 3. Funnel plots of variance ratio.

Funnel plots of variance ratio between arms with the 208 studies (Panel A) and for over-time comparison with the 95 studies for which the variance of the difference between the basal and final response was available (Panel B). Vertical axis indicates precision for the comparison of variances; with points outside the triangle being statistically significant. Additionally, red points mark significant differences between the means, which correspond to the objective of each study to assess main treatment effects. In Panel A, points on the right indicate higher outcome variability for the treated individuals, as expected if there is patient-by-treatment interaction; similarly, points on the left correspond to lower variability, although this is compatible with traditional Evidence-Based Medicine. Eleven (5.2%) out of 208 studies reported exactly the same outcome variability in both arms. We observe more red points on the left, indicating that changes in the average, come with reductions in the variance. In Panel B, points on the right indicate higher variability in the experimental arm at the end of the study, as expected in a scenario of heterogeneous treatment effect; points on the left correspond to lower variability at the end, which implies a more homogenous response after treatment. The largest number of points on the left side indicates a majority of experimental interventions that reduce variability. In addition, several of these interventions yielded significant results in the main endpoint.

Subgroup analyses suggest that only significant interventions had an effect on reducing variability (Supplementary File 1 : Figures S3–S5), which has already been observed in other studies22,23 and in the line of other works that had found a positive correlation between the effect size and its heterogeneity24,25: in fact, it is difficult to find heterogeneity when there is no overall treatment effect. The remaining subgroups analyses did not raise concerns (section V in Supplementary File 1).

Discussion

Our main objective was to show that comparing variances can provide some evidence about how much precision medicine is needed. The variability seems to decrease for treatments that perform significantly better than the reference; otherwise, it remains similar. Therefore, contrary to popular belief, variability tends to be reduced on average after treatment, thus making precision medicine dispensable in most cases. This could be due to several reasons: some measurements may have “ceiling” or “floor” effects (e.g., in the extreme case, if a treatment makes a person well, no further improvement is possible); or the treatment may act proportionally rather than linearly, in which case the logarithm of the outcome would serve as a better scale.

When both arms have equal variances, then an obvious default explanation is that the treatment is equally effective for all, thus rendering the search for predictors of differential response futile. This means that treatment effects obtained by comparing the means between groups can be used to estimate both the averaged treatment effect and the non-observable patient treatment effect.

Furthermore, our second objective was to provide a rough estimate of the proportion of interventions that require a greater degree of precision medicine, and our answer is “not many”: considering the most extreme result from Table 1, we found that 1/6 interventions (16.8%) showed some variance inflation.

There are three reasons why these findings do not invalidate precision medicine in all settings. First, some additional heterogeneity is present in the outcome variances ratio, which indicates that the variability had increased between arms as well as over time. Second, the outcomes of some type of interventions such as surgeries, for example, are greatly influenced by the skills and training of those administering the intervention, and these situations could have some effect on increasing variability. And third, we focus on quantitative outcomes, which are neither time-to-event nor binary, meaning that the effect could take a different form, such as all-or-nothing.

The results rely on published articles, which raises some relevant issues. First, some of our analyses are based on Normality assumptions for the outcomes that are unverifiable without access to raw data. Second, a high number of manuscripts (61.6%, Figure 2) act contrary to CONSORT26 advice in that they do not report variability. Thus, the results may be biased in either direction. Third, trials are usually powered to test constant effects and thus the presence of greater variability would lead to underpowered trials, non-significant results and unpublished papers. Fourth, the random effect model reveals additional heterogeneity in the outcome variances ratio, which may be the result of methodological inaccuracies20 arising from typographical errors in data translation, inadequate follow-up, insufficient reporting, or even data fabrication. On the other hand, this heterogeneity could also be the result of relevant undetected factors interacting with the treatment, which would indeed justify the need for precision medicine. A fifth limitation is that many clinical trials are not completely randomized. For example, multicenter trials are often blocked by center through the permuted blocks method. This means that if variances are calculated as if the trial were completely randomized (which is standard practice), the standard simple theory covering the random variation of variances from arm to arm is at best approximately true22

The main limitation of our study arises from the fact that, although constant effect always implies homoscedasticity on the chosen scale, the reverse is not true; i.e., homoscedasticity does not necessarily imply a constant effect. Nevertheless, a constant effect is the simplest explanation for homoscedasticity. For example, the non-parsimonious situation reflected in Figure 4 indicates homoscedasticity but without a constant effect.

c35fa272-0759-480c-8618-794dfb9254ab_figure4.gif

Figure 4. Scenario representing a fictional trial with 8 participants with homoscedasticity but non constant effect.

SBP potential values of each patient in both groups (C: Control; T: Treated) under a highly hypothetical scenario: the treatment effect has no value if systematically applied to the whole population; but if n-of-1 trials could be performed in this situation, the best treatment strategy would be chosen for each patient and the overall health of the population would be improved.

Heteroscedasticity may suggest the need for further refinements of the eligibility criteria or for finding an additive scale22,27. Because interaction analyses cannot include unknown variables, all trials would potentially need to be repeated once any new potential interaction variable emerges (e.g., a new biomarker) as a candidate for a new subgroup analysis. Nevertheless, we have shown how homoscedasticity can be assessed when reporting trials with numerical outcomes, regardless of whether every potential effect modifier is known.

For most trials, subjects vary little in their response to treatment, which suggests that precision medicine’s scope may be less than what is commonly assumed. In the past century, Evidence-Based Medicine operated under the paradigm of a constant effect assumption, by which we learned from previous patients in order to develop practical clinical guides for treating future ones. Here, we have provided empirical insights for the rationale behind Evidence-Based Medicine. However, even where one common effect applies to all patients fulfilling the eligibility criteria, this does not imply the same decision is optimal for all patients, specifically because different patients and stakeholders may vary in their weighting not only of efficacy outcomes, but also of the harm and cost of the interventions – thus bridging the gap between common evidence and personalized decisions.

Nevertheless, in 16 trials of our sample, there was some evidence of variation arising from the treatment effect, suggesting a possible role for more tailored treatments: either with finer selection criteria (common effect within specific subgroups), or with n-of-1 trials (no subgroups of patients with a common effect). By identifying indications where the scope for precision medicine is limited, studies such as ours may free up resources for cases with a greater scope.

Our results uphold the assertion by Horwitz et al. that there is a “need to measure a greater range of features to determine [...] the response to treatment”28. One of these features is an old friend of statisticians: the variance. Looking only at averages can cause us to miss out on important information.

Data availability

Data is available through two sources:

In both sources, the data can be downloaded under a Creative Commons License v. 4.0.

The code for the main analysis is available in the following link: https://doi.org/10.5281/zenodo.113360919

Comments on this article Comments (1)

Version 5
VERSION 5 PUBLISHED 10 Jun 2019
Revised
Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 09 Jan 2018
Discussion is closed on this version, please comment on the latest version above.
  • Reader Comment 16 Feb 2018
    Jake Westfall, PotentiaMetrics, USA
    16 Feb 2018
    Reader Comment
    Interesting approach. It seems to me that the limitation dismissed in/around Figure 4 is more serious than is let on. The contrived figure makes it seem like an exotic scenario, ... Continue reading
  • Discussion is closed on this version, please comment on the latest version above.
Author details Author details
Competing interests
Grant information
Copyright
Download
 
Export To
metrics
Views Downloads
F1000Research - -
PubMed Central
Data from PMC are received and updated monthly.
- -
Citations
CITE
how to cite this article
Cortés J, González JA, Medina MN et al. Does evidence support the high expectations placed in precision medicine? A bibliographic review [version 1; peer review: 2 approved with reservations, 1 not approved]. F1000Research 2018, 7:30 (https://doi.org/10.12688/f1000research.13490.1)
NOTE: If applicable, it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
track
receive updates on this article
Track an article to receive email alerts on any updates to this article.

Open Peer Review

Current Reviewer Status: ?
Key to Reviewer Statuses VIEW
ApprovedThe paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approvedFundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Version 1
VERSION 1
PUBLISHED 09 Jan 2018
Views
47
Cite
Reviewer Report 03 Apr 2018
Saskia le Cessie, Department of Clinical Epidemiology, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, The Netherlands;  Section Medical Statistics, Department of Biomedical Data Science, Leiden University Medical Center, Leiden, The Netherlands 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 47
Review report. Does evidence support the high expectations placed in precision medicine? A bibliographic review. By J Cortés et al,

Summary: a review of randomised trials with continuous outcomes, measured at baseline and at follow up has ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
le Cessie S. Reviewer Report For: Does evidence support the high expectations placed in precision medicine? A bibliographic review [version 1; peer review: 2 approved with reservations, 1 not approved]. F1000Research 2018, 7:30 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.14648.r31692)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 13 Jun 2018
    Jordi Cortés, Department of Statistics and Operations Research, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Barcelona, 08034, Spain
    13 Jun 2018
    Author Response
    JOINT ANSWER to Ian White and Saskia le Cessie

    This is a general response to Ian White and Saskia Le Cessie on why we stated that the standard clinical trial design ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 13 Jun 2018
    Jordi Cortés, Department of Statistics and Operations Research, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Barcelona, 08034, Spain
    13 Jun 2018
    Author Response
    JOINT ANSWER to Ian White and Saskia le Cessie

    This is a general response to Ian White and Saskia Le Cessie on why we stated that the standard clinical trial design ... Continue reading
Views
88
Cite
Reviewer Report 23 Mar 2018
Erica E.M. Moodie, Department of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Occupational Health, McGill University, Montreal, QC, Canada 
Not Approved
VIEWS 88
The authors have performed a review of a sample of clinical trials conducted every three years from 2004-2013 to examine whether there exists post-treatment heterogeneity in participants responses with premise that lack of heterogeneity suggests that precision medicine is not ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
Moodie EEM. Reviewer Report For: Does evidence support the high expectations placed in precision medicine? A bibliographic review [version 1; peer review: 2 approved with reservations, 1 not approved]. F1000Research 2018, 7:30 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.14648.r31694)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 04 Jun 2018
    Jordi Cortés, Department of Statistics and Operations Research, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Barcelona, 08034, Spain
    04 Jun 2018
    Author Response
    The referee’s objections can be summarized in two points:

    (1) “There are numerous examples of covariate-tailored treatment algorithms.”
    (2) “Randomized trials tend to be populated by homogenous populations”, which in turn does ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 04 Jun 2018
    Jordi Cortés, Department of Statistics and Operations Research, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Barcelona, 08034, Spain
    04 Jun 2018
    Author Response
    The referee’s objections can be summarized in two points:

    (1) “There are numerous examples of covariate-tailored treatment algorithms.”
    (2) “Randomized trials tend to be populated by homogenous populations”, which in turn does ... Continue reading
Views
59
Cite
Reviewer Report 02 Mar 2018
Ian R. White, Medical Research Council Clinical Trials Unit, Institute of Clinical Trials and Methodology, University College London, London, UK 
Approved with Reservations
VIEWS 59
This paper considers randomised trials (RCTs) of treatment versus control with a quantitative outcome. It observes that if treatment effects are homogeneous (the same for all trial participants) then the outcome variance will be the same in both trial arms. ... Continue reading
CITE
CITE
HOW TO CITE THIS REPORT
White IR. Reviewer Report For: Does evidence support the high expectations placed in precision medicine? A bibliographic review [version 1; peer review: 2 approved with reservations, 1 not approved]. F1000Research 2018, 7:30 (https://doi.org/10.5256/f1000research.14648.r30604)
NOTE: it is important to ensure the information in square brackets after the title is included in all citations of this article.
  • Author Response 13 Jun 2018
    Jordi Cortés, Department of Statistics and Operations Research, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Barcelona, 08034, Spain
    13 Jun 2018
    Author Response
    JOINT ANSWER to Ian White and Saskia le Cessie

    This is a general response to Ian White and Saskia Le Cessie on why we stated that the standard clinical trial design ... Continue reading
  • Author Response 15 Nov 2018
    Jordi Cortés, Department of Statistics and Operations Research, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Barcelona, 08034, Spain
    15 Nov 2018
    Author Response
    Following your suggestions together with those of the other reviewers, we have updated the manuscript with a new version that aims to emphasize the fact that researchers’ assumption of a ... Continue reading
COMMENTS ON THIS REPORT
  • Author Response 13 Jun 2018
    Jordi Cortés, Department of Statistics and Operations Research, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Barcelona, 08034, Spain
    13 Jun 2018
    Author Response
    JOINT ANSWER to Ian White and Saskia le Cessie

    This is a general response to Ian White and Saskia Le Cessie on why we stated that the standard clinical trial design ... Continue reading
  • Author Response 15 Nov 2018
    Jordi Cortés, Department of Statistics and Operations Research, Universitat Politècnica de Catalunya, Barcelona, 08034, Spain
    15 Nov 2018
    Author Response
    Following your suggestions together with those of the other reviewers, we have updated the manuscript with a new version that aims to emphasize the fact that researchers’ assumption of a ... Continue reading

Comments on this article Comments (1)

Version 5
VERSION 5 PUBLISHED 10 Jun 2019
Revised
Version 1
VERSION 1 PUBLISHED 09 Jan 2018
Discussion is closed on this version, please comment on the latest version above.
  • Reader Comment 16 Feb 2018
    Jake Westfall, PotentiaMetrics, USA
    16 Feb 2018
    Reader Comment
    Interesting approach. It seems to me that the limitation dismissed in/around Figure 4 is more serious than is let on. The contrived figure makes it seem like an exotic scenario, ... Continue reading
  • Discussion is closed on this version, please comment on the latest version above.
Alongside their report, reviewers assign a status to the article:
Approved - the paper is scientifically sound in its current form and only minor, if any, improvements are suggested
Approved with reservations - A number of small changes, sometimes more significant revisions are required to address specific details and improve the papers academic merit.
Not approved - fundamental flaws in the paper seriously undermine the findings and conclusions
Sign In
If you've forgotten your password, please enter your email address below and we'll send you instructions on how to reset your password.

The email address should be the one you originally registered with F1000.

Email address not valid, please try again

You registered with F1000 via Google, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Google account password, please click here.

You registered with F1000 via Facebook, so we cannot reset your password.

To sign in, please click here.

If you still need help with your Facebook account password, please click here.

Code not correct, please try again
Email us for further assistance.
Server error, please try again.